Amusement: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m Journal cites, Added 15 dois to journal cites
Line 6: Line 6:
'''Amusement''', from the old French ''à muser'' – to put into a stupid stare,<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/6786#eid4700112|title=Amuse, v.|last=|first=|date=|website=Oxford English Dictionary|archive-url=|archive-date=|dead-url=|accessdate=21 November 2017}}</ref> is the state of [[experience|experiencing]] [[humour|humorous]] and [[entertainment|entertaining]] events or situations while the person or animal actively maintains the experience, and is associated with [[enjoyment]], [[happiness]], [[laughter]] and [[pleasure]]. It is an emotion with positive valence and high physiological arousal.
'''Amusement''', from the old French ''à muser'' – to put into a stupid stare,<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/6786#eid4700112|title=Amuse, v.|last=|first=|date=|website=Oxford English Dictionary|archive-url=|archive-date=|dead-url=|accessdate=21 November 2017}}</ref> is the state of [[experience|experiencing]] [[humour|humorous]] and [[entertainment|entertaining]] events or situations while the person or animal actively maintains the experience, and is associated with [[enjoyment]], [[happiness]], [[laughter]] and [[pleasure]]. It is an emotion with positive valence and high physiological arousal.


Amusement is considered an "epistemological" emotion because humor occurs when one experiences a cognitive shift from one knowledge structure about a target to another, such as hearing the punchline of a joke.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P.|title=Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style.|journal=The Journal of Positive Psychology|date=2006|volume=1|issue=2|pages=61–71}}</ref> The pleasant surprise that happens from learning this new information leads to a state of amusement which people often express through smiling, laughter or chuckling.
Amusement is considered an "epistemological" emotion because humor occurs when one experiences a cognitive shift from one knowledge structure about a target to another, such as hearing the punchline of a joke.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P.|title=Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style.|journal=The Journal of Positive Psychology|date=2006|volume=1|issue=2|pages=61–71|doi=10.1080/17439760500510833}}</ref> The pleasant surprise that happens from learning this new information leads to a state of amusement which people often express through smiling, laughter or chuckling.


Current studies have not yet reached consensus on the exact purpose of amusement, though theories have been advanced in the fields of psychology, psychiatry and sociology. In addition, the precise mechanism that causes a given element (image, sound, behavior, etc.) to be perceived as more or less 'amusing' than another, similar element to a particular individual is not clearly understood.
Current studies have not yet reached consensus on the exact purpose of amusement, though theories have been advanced in the fields of psychology, psychiatry and sociology. In addition, the precise mechanism that causes a given element (image, sound, behavior, etc.) to be perceived as more or less 'amusing' than another, similar element to a particular individual is not clearly understood.
Line 18: Line 18:


=== Constructed emotion ===
=== Constructed emotion ===
The theory of constructed emotion suggests that when humans have a positive experience that increases their arousal, such as a faster heartbeat and increased sweat production, they cognitively seek out a label for that feeling.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Barrett, L. F.|title=Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion|journal=Personality and social psychology review|date=2006|volume=10|issue=1|pages=20–46}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Barrett, L. F.|title=The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization|journal=Social cognitive and affective neuroscience|volume=12|issue=1|pages=1–23}}</ref> They will land on amusement if that experience resembles other amusement experiences they have had.{{cn|date=December 2017}}
The theory of constructed emotion suggests that when humans have a positive experience that increases their arousal, such as a faster heartbeat and increased sweat production, they cognitively seek out a label for that feeling.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Barrett, L. F.|title=Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion|journal=Personality and social psychology review|date=2006|volume=10|issue=1|pages=20–46|doi=10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Barrett, L. F.|title=The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization|journal=Social cognitive and affective neuroscience|volume=12|issue=1|pages=1–23}}</ref> They will land on amusement if that experience resembles other amusement experiences they have had.{{cn|date=December 2017}}


== Emotional expression of amusement ==
== Emotional expression of amusement ==
The origin of the study of emotional expression is often attributed to [[Charles Darwin]].<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Keltner, D., Tracy, J. L., Sauter, D. A., Cordaro, D. C. & McNeil, G.|title=Expression of Emotion.| journal=In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. Eds., Handbook of Emotions|date=2016|volume=4th|pages=467–482}}</ref> In 1872, Darwin published his book ''[[The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals]]'', in which he extensively describes his observations of how people and animals display emotions on their faces and through sound.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Darwin|first1=Charles|title=The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals|orig-year=1872|year=1998|publisher=Harper Collins|location=London|edition=3}}</ref> Darwin comments on amusement as an emotion of great joy during which a person is likely to laugh. He also notes that across all peoples humans can laugh until they are panting and tears roll down their cheeks. In the 1960s, [[Paul Ekman]], an American Psychologist, revived the study of emotional expression proposing, like Darwin, that emotions were universally recognizable.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Ekman|first1=Paul|title=The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions|journal=Handbook of social psychophysiology|date=1989|pages=143–164}}</ref> His early work focused on six basic emotions: [[happiness]], [[anger]], [[sadness]], [[fear]], [[disgust]] and [[surprise (emotion)|surprise]].<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.|title=Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion|journal=Science|date=1969|volume=164|issue=3875|pages=86–88}}</ref> By the 1990s, he had expanded this list to include several additional emotions, one of which was amusement.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (Eds.).|title=What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS)|date=1997|publisher=Oxford University Press, USA.}}</ref> Displays of amusement have been distinguished from related emotions like embarrassment and shame.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Keltner|first1=Dacher|title=Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=1995|volume=68|issue=3|pages=441–454}}</ref> More recent studies have confirmed that laughter is a distinct signal of amusement and is recognizable across cultures.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A.|title=The voice conveys specific emotions: evidence from vocal burst displays|journal=Emotion|date=2009|volume=9 |issue=6|pages=838–846}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Laukka, P., Elfenbein, H. A., Söder, N., Nordström, H., Althoff, J., Chui, W., ... & Thingujam, N. S.|title=Cross-cultural decoding of positive and negative non-linguistic emotion vocalizations.|journal=Frontiers in Psychology,|date=2013|volume=4|pages=353}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010).|title=Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|date=2010|volume=107|issue=6|pages=2408–2412}}</ref>
The origin of the study of emotional expression is often attributed to [[Charles Darwin]].<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Keltner, D., Tracy, J. L., Sauter, D. A., Cordaro, D. C. & McNeil, G.|title=Expression of Emotion.| journal=In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. Eds., Handbook of Emotions|date=2016|volume=4th|pages=467–482}}</ref> In 1872, Darwin published his book ''[[The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals]]'', in which he extensively describes his observations of how people and animals display emotions on their faces and through sound.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Darwin|first1=Charles|title=The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals|orig-year=1872|year=1998|publisher=Harper Collins|location=London|edition=3}}</ref> Darwin comments on amusement as an emotion of great joy during which a person is likely to laugh. He also notes that across all peoples humans can laugh until they are panting and tears roll down their cheeks. In the 1960s, [[Paul Ekman]], an American Psychologist, revived the study of emotional expression proposing, like Darwin, that emotions were universally recognizable.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Ekman|first1=Paul|title=The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions|journal=Handbook of social psychophysiology|date=1989|pages=143–164}}</ref> His early work focused on six basic emotions: [[happiness]], [[anger]], [[sadness]], [[fear]], [[disgust]] and [[surprise (emotion)|surprise]].<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V.|title=Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion|journal=Science|date=1969|volume=164|issue=3875|pages=86–88|doi=10.1126/science.164.3875.86}}</ref> By the 1990s, he had expanded this list to include several additional emotions, one of which was amusement.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (Eds.).|title=What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS)|date=1997|publisher=Oxford University Press, USA.}}</ref> Displays of amusement have been distinguished from related emotions like embarrassment and shame.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Keltner|first1=Dacher|title=Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=1995|volume=68|issue=3|pages=441–454|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.441}}</ref> More recent studies have confirmed that laughter is a distinct signal of amusement and is recognizable across cultures.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A.|title=The voice conveys specific emotions: evidence from vocal burst displays|journal=Emotion|date=2009|volume=9 |issue=6|pages=838–846|doi=10.1037/a0017810}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Laukka, P., Elfenbein, H. A., Söder, N., Nordström, H., Althoff, J., Chui, W., ... & Thingujam, N. S.|title=Cross-cultural decoding of positive and negative non-linguistic emotion vocalizations.|journal=Frontiers in Psychology,|date=2013|volume=4|pages=353|doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00353}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last1=Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010).|title=Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations|journal=Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences|date=2010|volume=107|issue=6|pages=2408–2412|doi=10.1073/pnas.0908239106}}</ref>


[[File:Ron Killings & Vince McMahon laughing.jpg|thumb|Amusement facial expressions]]
[[File:Ron Killings & Vince McMahon laughing.jpg|thumb|Amusement facial expressions]]
Line 36: Line 36:
== Cultural considerations ==
== Cultural considerations ==


As a positive, high arousal emotion, amusement falls in the same category as excitement and exhilaration. These emotions are highly valued in American culture where positive feelings that high in energy are seen as ideal. In contrast, East Asian cultures value positive, low arousal emotions such as contentment, calm and peacefulness.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tsai|first1=Jeanne L.|title=Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences.|journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science|date=2007|volume=2|issue=3|pages=242–259}}</ref>
As a positive, high arousal emotion, amusement falls in the same category as excitement and exhilaration. These emotions are highly valued in American culture where positive feelings that high in energy are seen as ideal. In contrast, East Asian cultures value positive, low arousal emotions such as contentment, calm and peacefulness.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tsai|first1=Jeanne L.|title=Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences.|journal=Perspectives on Psychological Science|date=2007|volume=2|issue=3|pages=242–259|doi=10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x}}</ref>


==Clinic uses==
==Clinic uses==
=== Emotion regulation ===
=== Emotion regulation ===


Emotion regulation is the term for how people attempt to influence - increasing, decreasing, maintaining or changing - how, when and where they experience emotion.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gross|first1=James J.|title=The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review|journal=Review of general psychology|date=1998|volume=2|issue=3|pages=271–299}}</ref> Through changing how they think about a target of amusement, like a funny video clip, people are able to increase and decrease how much amusement they feel, express in smiles and laughs and experience in their bodies (e.g. increased heart rate and respiration).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Giuliani, N. R., McRae, K., & Gross, J. J.|title=The up-and down-regulation of amusement: experiential, behavioral, and autonomic consequences|journal=Emotion|date=2008|volume=8|issue=5|pages=714–719}}</ref> Choosing to increase or prolong experiences of positive emotion is one way in which people can learn to cope when faced with challenges and develop resilience.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L.|title=Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience|journal=Journal of Happiness Studies|date=2007|volume=8|issue=3|pages=311–333}}</ref>
Emotion regulation is the term for how people attempt to influence - increasing, decreasing, maintaining or changing - how, when and where they experience emotion.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gross|first1=James J.|title=The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review|journal=Review of general psychology|date=1998|volume=2|issue=3|pages=271–299|doi=10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271}}</ref> Through changing how they think about a target of amusement, like a funny video clip, people are able to increase and decrease how much amusement they feel, express in smiles and laughs and experience in their bodies (e.g. increased heart rate and respiration).<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Giuliani, N. R., McRae, K., & Gross, J. J.|title=The up-and down-regulation of amusement: experiential, behavioral, and autonomic consequences|journal=Emotion|date=2008|volume=8|issue=5|pages=714–719|doi=10.1037/a0013236}}</ref> Choosing to increase or prolong experiences of positive emotion is one way in which people can learn to cope when faced with challenges and develop resilience.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L.|title=Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience|journal=Journal of Happiness Studies|date=2007|volume=8|issue=3|pages=311–333|doi=10.1007/s10902-006-9015-4}}</ref>


=== Resilience ===
=== Resilience ===


As a positive emotion, amusement contributes to the development of resilience in both children<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bai, S., & Repetti, R. L.|title=Short‐term resilience processes in the family.|journal=Family relations|date=2015|volume=64|issue=1|pages=108–119}}</ref> and adults.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R.|title=What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=2003|volume=84|issue=2|pages=365–376}}</ref> Positive emotions help people build social resources that foster their ability to cope during hard times.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2004).|title=Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health.|journal=Journal of personality|date=2004|volume=72|issue=6|pages=1161–1190}}</ref> Increases in resilience lead to higher [[life satisfaction]] and general happiness.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Cohn|first=Michael A.|last2=Fredrickson|first2=Barbara L.|last3=Brown|first3=Stephanie L.|last4=Mikels|first4=Joseph A.|last5=Conway|first5=Anne M.|date=2009|title=Happiness Unpacked: Positive Emotions Increase Life Satisfaction by Building Resilience|journal=Emotion (Washington, D.C.)|volume=9|issue=3|pages=361–368|doi=10.1037/a0015952|issn=1528-3542|pmc=3126102|pmid=19485613}}</ref> Studies of positive emotion typically include measures of amusement, but do not assess for its unique impact by differentiating between the effects of discrete positive emotions like gratitude, love and compassion.{{cn|date=December 2017}}
As a positive emotion, amusement contributes to the development of resilience in both children<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Bai, S., & Repetti, R. L.|title=Short‐term resilience processes in the family.|journal=Family relations|date=2015|volume=64|issue=1|pages=108–119|doi=10.1111/fare.12101}}</ref> and adults.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R.|title=What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001|journal=Journal of Personality and Social Psychology|date=2003|volume=84|issue=2|pages=365–376|doi=10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365}}</ref> Positive emotions help people build social resources that foster their ability to cope during hard times.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2004).|title=Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health.|journal=Journal of personality|date=2004|volume=72|issue=6|pages=1161–1190|doi=10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x}}</ref> Increases in resilience lead to higher [[life satisfaction]] and general happiness.<ref>{{Cite journal|last=Cohn|first=Michael A.|last2=Fredrickson|first2=Barbara L.|last3=Brown|first3=Stephanie L.|last4=Mikels|first4=Joseph A.|last5=Conway|first5=Anne M.|date=2009|title=Happiness Unpacked: Positive Emotions Increase Life Satisfaction by Building Resilience|journal=Emotion (Washington, D.C.)|volume=9|issue=3|pages=361–368|doi=10.1037/a0015952|issn=1528-3542|pmc=3126102|pmid=19485613}}</ref> Studies of positive emotion typically include measures of amusement, but do not assess for its unique impact by differentiating between the effects of discrete positive emotions like gratitude, love and compassion.{{cn|date=December 2017}}
[[File:Laughter Yoga.jpg|thumb|People engaging in laughter therapy]]
[[File:Laughter Yoga.jpg|thumb|People engaging in laughter therapy]]


=== Laughter therapy ===
=== Laughter therapy ===


Rapidly growing in popularity, [[laughter|laughter therapy]], or humor therapy, focuses on promoting laughter as a therapeutic tool.<ref>{{cite web|last1=Burbank|first1=Luke|title=The Growing Popularity of Laughter Therapy|url=https://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=5165226|website=NPR.org|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> Usually implemented in a group, laughter therapy uses a variety of gag jokes like red noses and magic tricks to encourage laughter - fake or real. Laughter causes the bodies' muscles to relax which increases blood flow and oxygen intake.<ref>{{cite web|title=Laughter Therapy|url=https://www.cancercenter.com/treatments/laughter-therapy/|website=Cancer Treatment Centers of America|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> Proposed benefits include: Stress management,<ref name="MayoClinicLaughter">{{cite web|last1=Mayo Clinic Staff|title=Stress relief from laughter? It's no joke|url=https://www.mayoclinic.org/healthy-lifestyle/stress-management/in-depth/stress-relief/art-20044456|website=www.mayoclinic.org|publisher=Mayo Clinic|accessdate=5 February 2018}}</ref> relaxation, improved mental functions, improved digestion and pain relief. Some suggest that laughter activates similar brain wave frequencies as a meditative state.<ref>{{cite web|last1=Eckelkamp|first1=Stephanie|title=Laughter Therapy is the New Meditation|url=http://time.com/84987/laughter-may-be-the-new-meditation/|website=Time.com|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> However, scientific studies of these benefits are limited and have methodological issues.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Martin, R. A.|title=Humor, laughter, and physical health: Methodological issues and research findings.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|date=2001|volume=127|issue=4|pages=504–519}}</ref>
Rapidly growing in popularity, [[laughter|laughter therapy]], or humor therapy, focuses on promoting laughter as a therapeutic tool.<ref>{{cite web|last1=Burbank|first1=Luke|title=The Growing Popularity of Laughter Therapy|url=https://www.npr.org/templates/story/story.php?storyId=5165226|website=NPR.org|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> Usually implemented in a group, laughter therapy uses a variety of gag jokes like red noses and magic tricks to encourage laughter - fake or real. Laughter causes the bodies' muscles to relax which increases blood flow and oxygen intake.<ref>{{cite web|title=Laughter Therapy|url=https://www.cancercenter.com/treatments/laughter-therapy/|website=Cancer Treatment Centers of America|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> Proposed benefits include: Stress management,<ref name="MayoClinicLaughter">{{cite web|last1=Mayo Clinic Staff|title=Stress relief from laughter? It's no joke|url=https://www.mayoclinic.org/healthy-lifestyle/stress-management/in-depth/stress-relief/art-20044456|website=www.mayoclinic.org|publisher=Mayo Clinic|accessdate=5 February 2018}}</ref> relaxation, improved mental functions, improved digestion and pain relief. Some suggest that laughter activates similar brain wave frequencies as a meditative state.<ref>{{cite web|last1=Eckelkamp|first1=Stephanie|title=Laughter Therapy is the New Meditation|url=http://time.com/84987/laughter-may-be-the-new-meditation/|website=Time.com|accessdate=24 November 2017}}</ref> However, scientific studies of these benefits are limited and have methodological issues.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Martin, R. A.|title=Humor, laughter, and physical health: Methodological issues and research findings.|journal=Psychological Bulletin|date=2001|volume=127|issue=4|pages=504–519|doi=10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.504}}</ref>


==References==
==References==

Revision as of 18:53, 9 February 2018

Amusement, Viktor Vasnetsov

Amusement, from the old French à muser – to put into a stupid stare,[1] is the state of experiencing humorous and entertaining events or situations while the person or animal actively maintains the experience, and is associated with enjoyment, happiness, laughter and pleasure. It is an emotion with positive valence and high physiological arousal.

Amusement is considered an "epistemological" emotion because humor occurs when one experiences a cognitive shift from one knowledge structure about a target to another, such as hearing the punchline of a joke.[2] The pleasant surprise that happens from learning this new information leads to a state of amusement which people often express through smiling, laughter or chuckling.

Current studies have not yet reached consensus on the exact purpose of amusement, though theories have been advanced in the fields of psychology, psychiatry and sociology. In addition, the precise mechanism that causes a given element (image, sound, behavior, etc.) to be perceived as more or less 'amusing' than another, similar element to a particular individual is not clearly understood.

Theories

Two different theoretical perspectives about emotion provide contrasting origins of amusement:

Evolutionary

The evolutionary perspective proposes that amusement is a distinct emotion that evolved to enhance a species’ survival. Collective laughter helps define an ingroup, helping people recognize those similar to them and feel included.[3][4] It also helps identify outgroup members and enhance the barrier between the two. Laughter can provide network support as encouragement, or it can inform ingroup members that they are losing majority favor and to adjust their behavior to reassimilate.[citation needed]

Constructed emotion

The theory of constructed emotion suggests that when humans have a positive experience that increases their arousal, such as a faster heartbeat and increased sweat production, they cognitively seek out a label for that feeling.[5][6] They will land on amusement if that experience resembles other amusement experiences they have had.[citation needed]

Emotional expression of amusement

The origin of the study of emotional expression is often attributed to Charles Darwin.[7] In 1872, Darwin published his book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, in which he extensively describes his observations of how people and animals display emotions on their faces and through sound.[8] Darwin comments on amusement as an emotion of great joy during which a person is likely to laugh. He also notes that across all peoples humans can laugh until they are panting and tears roll down their cheeks. In the 1960s, Paul Ekman, an American Psychologist, revived the study of emotional expression proposing, like Darwin, that emotions were universally recognizable.[9] His early work focused on six basic emotions: happiness, anger, sadness, fear, disgust and surprise.[10] By the 1990s, he had expanded this list to include several additional emotions, one of which was amusement.[11] Displays of amusement have been distinguished from related emotions like embarrassment and shame.[12] More recent studies have confirmed that laughter is a distinct signal of amusement and is recognizable across cultures.[13][14][15]

Amusement facial expressions

Facial expression

An amused facial expression typically has these characteristics:[16]

  • Head: Thrown back with jaw lifted
  • Eyes: Crow's feet at the eyes indicating that the muscles have tightened
  • Mouth: Open, jaw dropped with relaxed muscles

Vocal burst

The expression of amusement is usually accompanied by genuine laughter. The experience of laughter changes our breathing pattern and often causes all our muscles to relax.[16]

Cultural considerations

As a positive, high arousal emotion, amusement falls in the same category as excitement and exhilaration. These emotions are highly valued in American culture where positive feelings that high in energy are seen as ideal. In contrast, East Asian cultures value positive, low arousal emotions such as contentment, calm and peacefulness.[17]

Clinic uses

Emotion regulation

Emotion regulation is the term for how people attempt to influence - increasing, decreasing, maintaining or changing - how, when and where they experience emotion.[18] Through changing how they think about a target of amusement, like a funny video clip, people are able to increase and decrease how much amusement they feel, express in smiles and laughs and experience in their bodies (e.g. increased heart rate and respiration).[19] Choosing to increase or prolong experiences of positive emotion is one way in which people can learn to cope when faced with challenges and develop resilience.[20]

Resilience

As a positive emotion, amusement contributes to the development of resilience in both children[21] and adults.[22] Positive emotions help people build social resources that foster their ability to cope during hard times.[23] Increases in resilience lead to higher life satisfaction and general happiness.[24] Studies of positive emotion typically include measures of amusement, but do not assess for its unique impact by differentiating between the effects of discrete positive emotions like gratitude, love and compassion.[citation needed]

People engaging in laughter therapy

Laughter therapy

Rapidly growing in popularity, laughter therapy, or humor therapy, focuses on promoting laughter as a therapeutic tool.[25] Usually implemented in a group, laughter therapy uses a variety of gag jokes like red noses and magic tricks to encourage laughter - fake or real. Laughter causes the bodies' muscles to relax which increases blood flow and oxygen intake.[26] Proposed benefits include: Stress management,[27] relaxation, improved mental functions, improved digestion and pain relief. Some suggest that laughter activates similar brain wave frequencies as a meditative state.[28] However, scientific studies of these benefits are limited and have methodological issues.[29]

References

  1. ^ "Amuse, v." Oxford English Dictionary. Retrieved 21 November 2017. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |dead-url= (help)
  2. ^ Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). "Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style". The Journal of Positive Psychology. 1 (2): 61–71. doi:10.1080/17439760500510833.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  3. ^ Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., Keltner, D., & Hertenstein, M. J. (2004). Positive emotion and the regulation of interpersonal relationships in The Regulation of Emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Philippot, P. & Feldman, R. S. Eds. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. pp. 127–155.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ Latta, R. L. (1999). The basic humor process: A cognitive-shift theory and the case against incongruity. New York, NY: Mouton de Gruyter.
  5. ^ Barrett, L. F. (2006). "Solving the emotion paradox: Categorization and the experience of emotion". Personality and social psychology review. 10 (1): 20–46. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2.
  6. ^ Barrett, L. F. "The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization". Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 12 (1): 1–23.
  7. ^ Keltner, D., Tracy, J. L., Sauter, D. A., Cordaro, D. C. & McNeil, G. (2016). "Expression of Emotion". In Barrett, L. F., Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. Eds., Handbook of Emotions. 4th: 467–482.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Darwin, Charles (1998) [1872]. The Expressions of the Emotions in Man and Animals (3 ed.). London: Harper Collins.
  9. ^ Ekman, Paul (1989). "The argument and evidence about universals in facial expressions". Handbook of social psychophysiology: 143–164.
  10. ^ Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). "Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion". Science. 164 (3875): 86–88. doi:10.1126/science.164.3875.86.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (Eds.). (1997). What the face reveals: Basic and applied studies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). Oxford University Press, USA.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  12. ^ Keltner, Dacher (1995). "Signs of appeasement: Evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 68 (3): 441–454. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.441.
  13. ^ Simon-Thomas, E. R., Keltner, D. J., Sauter, D., Sinicropi-Yao, L., & Abramson, A. (2009). "The voice conveys specific emotions: evidence from vocal burst displays". Emotion. 9 (6): 838–846. doi:10.1037/a0017810.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  14. ^ Laukka, P., Elfenbein, H. A., Söder, N., Nordström, H., Althoff, J., Chui, W., ... & Thingujam, N. S. (2013). "Cross-cultural decoding of positive and negative non-linguistic emotion vocalizations". Frontiers in Psychology,. 4: 353. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00353.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: extra punctuation (link) CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  15. ^ Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P., & Scott, S. K. (2010). (2010). "Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (6): 2408–2412. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908239106.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ a b "Emotional Intelligence Quiz". Greater Good Science Center. Retrieved 23 November 2017.
  17. ^ Tsai, Jeanne L. (2007). "Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (3): 242–259. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x.
  18. ^ Gross, James J. (1998). "The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review". Review of general psychology. 2 (3): 271–299. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271.
  19. ^ Giuliani, N. R., McRae, K., & Gross, J. J. (2008). "The up-and down-regulation of amusement: experiential, behavioral, and autonomic consequences". Emotion. 8 (5): 714–719. doi:10.1037/a0013236.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2007). "Regulation of positive emotions: Emotion regulation strategies that promote resilience". Journal of Happiness Studies. 8 (3): 311–333. doi:10.1007/s10902-006-9015-4.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ Bai, S., & Repetti, R. L. (2015). "Short‐term resilience processes in the family". Family relations. 64 (1): 108–119. doi:10.1111/fare.12101.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  22. ^ Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). "What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (2): 365–376. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ Tugade, M. M., Fredrickson, B. L., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2004). (2004). "Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health". Journal of personality. 72 (6): 1161–1190. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  24. ^ Cohn, Michael A.; Fredrickson, Barbara L.; Brown, Stephanie L.; Mikels, Joseph A.; Conway, Anne M. (2009). "Happiness Unpacked: Positive Emotions Increase Life Satisfaction by Building Resilience". Emotion (Washington, D.C.). 9 (3): 361–368. doi:10.1037/a0015952. ISSN 1528-3542. PMC 3126102. PMID 19485613.
  25. ^ Burbank, Luke. "The Growing Popularity of Laughter Therapy". NPR.org. Retrieved 24 November 2017.
  26. ^ "Laughter Therapy". Cancer Treatment Centers of America. Retrieved 24 November 2017.
  27. ^ Mayo Clinic Staff. "Stress relief from laughter? It's no joke". www.mayoclinic.org. Mayo Clinic. Retrieved 5 February 2018.
  28. ^ Eckelkamp, Stephanie. "Laughter Therapy is the New Meditation". Time.com. Retrieved 24 November 2017.
  29. ^ Martin, R. A. (2001). "Humor, laughter, and physical health: Methodological issues and research findings". Psychological Bulletin. 127 (4): 504–519. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.504.

Further reading

  • Brockton Art Center (Brockton, Mass.). American Pastimes: [exhibition catalog], Brockton Art Center, Fuller Memorial, Brockton, Massachusetts, Jan. 27-Apr. 17, 1977. Roger T. Dunn ... curator [and author of the] catalogue; research assistant, Richard Campbell. Brockton, Mass.: Brockton Art Center, Fuller Memorial; Bedford, Mass.: printed by W.E. Andrews Co., 1976. 44 obl. p., amply ill. Without ISBN

External links