Jump to content

Sleep: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Replaced content with 'What y'all Nigga's call sleeep is wen us niggass are hidden from the coppers nd we qet a rest. for y'all white niggass its wen ever your tiredd u just lay your ...'
m Reverted edits by 71.125.91.182 to last version by Unpopular Opinion (HG)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Otheruses}}
What y'all Nigga's call sleeep is wen us niggass are hidden from the coppers nd we qet a rest. for y'all white niggass its wen ever your tiredd u just lay your lazyy ass down in a nice comfy bed. My Point Is Its when you REST..
{{redirect|Waking up||Waking Up}}
{{Copyedit|date=October 2008}}
[[Image:Sleepy men.JPG|thumb|300px|Sleepy men in [[Tehran]], [[Iran]]]]


'''Sleep''' is a [[natural]] state of [[body|bodily]] [[rest]] observed throughout the [[animal kingdom]]. It is common to all [[mammals]] and [[birds]], and is also seen in many [[reptiles]], [[amphibians]] and [[fish]]. In humans, other mammals, and a substantial majority of other animals which have been studied — such as fish, birds, ants, and [[Drosophilidae|fruit-flies]] — regular sleep is essential for survival. A widely publicized 2003 study performed at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.<ref> {{cite book |last= |first= |authorlink= |coauthors= |editor= |others= Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council |title= Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research |origdate= |origyear= |origmonth= |url= http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10732&page=121 |format= |accessdate= |accessyear= |accessmonth= |edition= |series= |volume= |date= |year= 2003 |month= |publisher= The National Academies Press |location= |isbn= 978-0-309-08903-6 |oclc= |doi= |id= |pages= pg 121 |chapter= |chapterurl= |quote= Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983).}}</ref> However, the purposes of sleep are only partly clear and are the subject of intense research.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://thesciencenetwork.org/programs/Waking%20Up%20To%20Sleep%202006/ |title= Waking Up To Sleep |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |author= |last= Bingham |first= Roger |authorlink= |coauthors= Terrence Sejnowski, Jerry Siegel, Mark Eric Dyken, Charles Czeisler, Paul Shaw, Ralph Greenspan, Satchin Panda, Philip Low, Robert Stickgold, Sara Mednick, Allan Pack, Luis de Lecea, David Dinges, Dan Kripke, Giulio Tononi |year= 2007 |month= February |format= Several conference videos |work= |publisher= The Science Network |pages= |language= |doi= |archiveurl= |archivedate= |quote= }}</ref>


== Physiology ==
In mammals and birds, sleep is divided into two broad types: [[Rapid eye movement sleep|Rapid Eye Movement]] (REM) and [[Non-rapid eye movement sleep|Non-Rapid Eye Movement]] (NREM) or "Non-REM" sleep. Each type has a distinct set of associated physiological, neurological and psychological features.


Sleep proceeds in cycles of REM and the three stages of NREM, the order normally being:
:''stages N1 -> N2 -> N3 -> N2 -> REM''.
[[File:Hypnogramme.svg|thumb|Sleep cycles through the night with deep sleep early on and more REM, marked in red, toward morning]]
(Until recently, NREM was considered to include four stages and the cycle was N1 -> N2 -> N3 -> N4 -> N3 -> N2 -> REM; this system is still seen in reference works and diagrams.)


In humans each sleep cycle lasts on average 90 to 110 minutes,<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.sleepdisorderchannel.com/stages/ |title= Sleep Stages. Overview, Waking, Non-REM, REM, Sleep Cycle, Factors, Age |accessdate= 2008-02-10 |last= Swierzewski |first= Stanley J., M.D. |date= 01 December 2000, reviewed 04 December 2007 |format= |work= |publisher= Sleep Channel, Healthcommunities.com |quote= }}</ref> with a greater amount of stage 3 (N3) early in the night and more REM later in the night. Each phase may have a distinct physiological function. Drugs such as [[sleeping pill]]s and [[alcoholic beverage]]s can suppress certain stages of sleep, leading to [[sleep deprivation]]. This can result in sleep that exhibits loss of consciousness but does not fulfill its physiological functions.


[[Allan Rechtschaffen]] and Anthony Kales originally outlined the criteria for identifying the stages of sleep in 1968. The [[American Academy of Sleep Medicine]] (AASM) updated the staging rules in 2007.


[[Image:SWS.jpg|thumb|165px|right|Stage 4 Sleep. [[EEG]] highlighted by red box.]]
Posted by: Black Niggas of America.
[[Image:REM.png|thumb|165px|right|[[Rapid eye movement (sleep)|REM sleep]]. EEG highlighted by red box. Eye movements highlighted by red line.]]

===Stages of sleep===
Sleep stages and other characteristics of sleep can be measured by [[polysomnography]] in a sleep laboratory using among other tools [[electroencephalography]] (EEG) for brain waves, [[electrooculography]] (EOG) for eye movements and electromyography (EMG) for activity of skeletal muscles.

Criteria for [[Rapid eye movement (sleep)|REM sleep]] include not only rapid eye movements but also rapid low voltage EEG, commonly called brain waves. In mammals, at least, low muscle tone is also seen, often called paralysis. Most memorable dreaming occurs in this stage. REM sleep accounts for 20–25% of total sleep time in normal human adults; NREM (non-REM) accounting for the rest. In NREM sleep, there is relatively little dreaming.

Non-REM encompasses three stages; stage 1 (N1), stage 2 (N2), and stage 3 (N3); N3 being referred to as 'deep sleep' or [[slow-wave sleep]], SWS. Sleep stages are differentiated by a combination of brain waves, eyes movements, and skeletal muscle activity. In non-REM sleep there may be limb movements, and [[parasomnia]]s such as [[sleepwalking]] may occur.

NREM consists of three stages according to the 2007 AASM standards:
* During '''Stage N1''' the brain transitions from [[alpha waves]] (having a frequency of 8 to 13 [[Hertz|Hz]], common to people who are awake) to [[theta rhythm|theta waves]] (with a frequency of 4 to 7 Hz). This stage is sometimes referred to as ''somnolence'', or "drowsy sleep". Associated with the onset of sleep during N1 may be sudden twitches and [[hypnic jerk]]s also known as positive [[myoclonus]]. Some people may also experience [[hypnagogic hallucination]]s during this stage, which can be troublesome to them. During N1 the subject loses some [[muscle tone]] and most conscious awareness of the external environment.
* '''Stage N2''', is characterized by "[[sleep spindle]]s" (12 to 16 Hz) and "[[K-complex]]es." During this stage, muscular activity as measured by EMG decreases and conscious awareness of the external environment disappears. This stage occupies 45 to 55% of total sleep.
* '''Stage N3''', deep or slow-wave sleep (SWS), is characterized by [[delta wave]]s, (0.5 to 4 Hz), also called ''delta rhythms''. This is the stage in which [[night terrors]], [[bedwetting]], [[sleepwalking]] and [[somniloquy|sleep-talking]] occur. In the previous system, the difference between stages 3 and 4 (now combined as 3) was that delta waves made up less than 50% of the total wave-patterns in stage 3, while they were more than 50% in stage 4.

* A recent ruling by the AASM discontinued stage 4 sleep (N4) and left only stage N3 to describe delta sleep.<ref name="Pinel, J.P.J.">{{cite web |url= http://web.mst.edu/~psyworld/general/sleepstages/sleepstages.pdf |title= Stages of Sleep |accessdate= 2008-06-15 |author= Psychology World |last= |first= |coauthors= |year= 1998 |format= PDF |work= |publisher= |archiveurl= |archivedate= |quote= }}</ref>

Both REM sleep and SWS sleep (previously stages 3 and 4, now combined as stage 3) are homeostatically driven; that is, people and most animals selectively deprived of one of these stages will rebound once uninhibited sleep is allowed. This finding suggests that both types of sleep are essential.

===Sleep timing===
Sleep timing is controlled by the [[Circadian rhythm|circadian clock]], by [[homeostasis]] and in humans, within certain bounds, by willed behavior. The circadian clock, an inner time-keeping, temperature-fluctuating, enzyme-controlling device, works in tandem with [[adenosine]], a neurotransmitter which inhibits many of the bodily processes that are associated with wakefulness. Adenosine is created over the course of the day; high levels of adenosine lead to sleepiness. In diurnal animals, sleepiness occurs as the circadian element causes the release of the hormone [[melatonin]] and a gradual decrease in core body temperature. The timing is affected by one's [[chronotype]]. It is the circadian rhythm which determines the ideal timing of a correctly structured and restorative sleep episode.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Wyatt |first=James K. |coauthors=Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan |year=1999 |month=October |title= Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day |journal=Am J Physiol |volume=277 |issue=4 |pages=R1152–R1163. Fulltext |id= |url= http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/277/4/R1152 | accessdate=2007-11-25 |quote= |pmid= 10516257 }}</ref>

Homeostatic sleep propensity, the need for sleep as a function of the amount of time elapsed since the last adequate sleep episode, is also important and must be balanced against the circadian element for satisfactory sleep. Along with corresponding messages from the circadian clock, this tells the body it needs to sleep.<ref name=autogenerated1>{{cite web |url= http://www.helpguide.org/life/sleeping.htm |title= Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |author= |last= de Benedictis |first= Tina, Ph.D. |coauthors= Heather Larson, Gina Kemp, M.A., Suzanne Barston, Robert Segal, M.A. |year= 2007 |publisher= Helpguide.org |quote= }}</ref> Sleep offset, awakening, is primarily determined by circadian rhythm. A normal person who regularly awakens at an early hour will generally not be able to sleep much later than the person's normal waking time, even if moderately sleep deprived.

=== Optimal amount in humans ===
==== Adults ====
The optimal amount of sleep is not a meaningful concept unless the timing of that sleep is seen in relation to an individual's [[circadian rhythm]]s. A person's major sleep episode is relatively inefficient and inadequate when it occurs at the "wrong" time of day. The timing is correct when the following two circadian markers occur after the middle of the sleep episode but before awakening:<ref>{{cite journal |last=Wyatt |first=James K. |coauthors=Ritz-De Cecco, Angela; Czeisler, Charles A.; Dijk, Derk-Jan |year=1999 |month=October |title= Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day |journal=Am J Physiol |volume=277 |issue=4 |pages=R1152–R1163 |id= |url= http://ajpregu.physiology.org/cgi/content/full/277/4/R1152
|accessdate=2007-11-25 |quote=... significant homeostatic and circadian modulation of sleep structure, with the highest sleep efficiency occurring in sleep episodes bracketing the melatonin maximum and core body temperature minimum |pmid= 10516257 }}</ref>
* maximum concentration of the hormone melatonin, and
* minimum core body temperature.

The [[National Sleep Foundation|National Sleep Foundation in the United States]] maintains that seven to nine hours of sleep for adult humans is optimal and that sufficient sleep benefits alertness, memory and problem solving, and overall health, as well as reducing the risk of accidents.<ref name="let sleep work for you">{{cite web | title = "Let Sleep Work for You" provided by the National Sleep Foundation | url = http://www.sleepfoundation.org/site/c.huIXKjM0IxF/b.2421185/k.7198/Let_Sleep_Work_for_You.htm}}</ref> A widely publicized 2003 study<ref>Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003 Mar 15;26(2):117–26.</ref> performed at the [[University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine]] demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.

However, a [[University of California, San Diego]] psychiatry study of more than one million adults found that people who live the longest self-report sleeping for six to seven hours each night.<ref>{{cite web | author = Rhonda Rowland | title = Experts challenge study linking sleep, life span |date=2002-02-15 | url = http://archives.cnn.com/2002/HEALTH/02/14/sleep.study/index.html | accessdate = 2007-04-22 }}</ref> Another study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women showed similar results.<ref>Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR et al (2004) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=15164896 A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women] ''Sleep'' 1;27(3):440-4</ref> Other studies show that "sleeping more than 7 to 8 hours per day has been consistently associated with increased mortality", though this study suggests the cause is probably other factors such as depression and socio-economic status which would correlate statistically. <ref>Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ et al (2006) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=pubmed&cmd=Retrieve&dopt=AbstractPlus&list_uids=16895254 Correlates of long sleep duration] ''Sleep'' 29(7):881-889</ref> It has been suggested that the correlation between lower sleep hours and reduced morbidity only occurs with those who wake after less sleep naturally, rather than those who use an alarm.

[[Image:Sleeping Kutchi.jpg|thumb|left|A [[Kutchi people|Kutchi woman]] sleeping in [[Nirona]] village]]
Causal links are currently speculative: the available data may only reflect comorbid depression, socioeconomic status, or even alcohol use, for example.<ref>Irwin MR, Ziegler M (2005) [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=pubmed&dopt=Abstract&list_uids=15642774 Sleep deprivation potentiates activation of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses in abstinent alcoholics] ''Hypertension'' 45(2):252-7</ref> These studies cannot be used to determine optimal sleep habits, only correlation — and [[correlation does not imply causation|an empirically observed correlation is a necessary but not sufficient condition for causality]]. A need for nine or ten hours of sleep a day, or only five to six, may or may not have the same cause as the shortened life span. In other words, long or short sleep duration itself has not been shown to be a cause of early death.

Researchers from the [[University of Warwick]] and [[University College London]] have found that lack of sleep can more than double the risk of death from [[cardiovascular disease]], but that too much sleep can also double the risk of death.<ref>{{cite web | title = "Researchers say lack of sleep doubles risk of death… but so can too much sleep" | url = http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/newsandevents/pressreleases/researchers_say_lack/}}</ref><ref>Jane E. Ferrie, Martin J. Shipley, Francesco P. Cappuccio, Eric Brunner, Michelle A. Miller, Meena Kumari, and Michael G. Marmot. “A prospective study of change in sleep duration; associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort”. SLEEP Journal.</ref> Professor Francesco Cappuccio said: “Short sleep has been shown to be a risk factor for weight gain, [[hypertension]] and Type 2 diabetes sometimes leading to mortality but in contrast to the short sleep-mortality association it appears that no potential mechanisms by which long sleep could be associated with increased mortality have yet been investigated. Some candidate causes for this include depression, low socioeconomic status and cancer-related fatigue. [...] In terms of prevention, our findings indicate that consistently sleeping around 7 hours per night is optimal for health and a sustained reduction may predispose to ill-health.”

Furthermore, sleep difficulties are closely associated with psychiatric disorders such as [[Major depressive disorder|depression]], [[alcoholism]] and [[bipolar disorder]]. Up to 90% of patients with depression are found to have sleep difficulties.{{Fact|date=October 2008}}

==== Hours by age ====
[[Image:A child sleeping.jpg|thumb|280px|A [[child]] sleeping]]

Children need a greater amount of sleep per day than adults to develop and function properly: up to 18 hours for [[newborn]] babies, with a declining rate as a child ages.<ref name="let sleep work for you" /><ref name=autogenerated1 /> A newborn baby spends almost half of its sleep time in REM-sleep. By the age of five or so, only a bit over two hours are spent in REM.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.npi.ucla.edu/sleepresearch/encarta/Article.htm | title= Sleep |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last= Siegel |first= Jerome M. |year= 1999 |format= |work= Encarta Encyclopedia |publisher= Microsoft |quote= }}</ref>

{| class="wikitable"
|-
! Age and condition
! Average amount of sleep per day
|-
| Newborn
| up to 18 hours
|-
| 1–12 months
| 14–18 hours
|-
| 1–3 years
| 12–15 hours
|-
| 3–5 years
| 11–13 hours
|-
| 5–12 years
| 9–11 hours
|-
| Adolescents
| 9-10
|-
| Adults, including elderly
| 7–8 (+) hours
|-
| Pregnant women
| 8 (+) hours
|}

=== Sleep debt ===
{{main|Sleep debt}}
Sleep debt is the effect of not getting quite enough rest and sleep; a large debt causes mental, emotional and physical fatigue. It is unclear why a lack of sleep causes irritability; however theories are emerging that suggest if the body produces insufficient cortisol during stage 3 and 4 sleep it can have negative effects on our alertness and emotions during the day.<ref>Zisapel N.(2007) Sleep and sleep disturbances: biological basis and clinical
implications. ''Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences'' </ref>{{Fact|date=October 2008}}

Scientists do not agree on how much sleep debt it is possible to accumulate, whether it is accumulated against an individual's average sleep or some other benchmark, nor on whether the prevalence of sleep debt among adults has changed appreciably in the industrialized world in recent decades. It is likely that children are sleeping less than previously in western societies.<ref>{{cite journal | last = Iglowstein | first = Ivo | coauthors = Oskar G. Jenni, MD, Luciano Molinari, PhD and Remo H. Largo, MD | year = 2003 | month = February | title = Sleep Duration From Infancy to Adolescence: Reference Values and Generational Trends | journal = Pediatrics | volume = 111 | issue = 2 | pages = 302–307 | publisher = | doi = 10.1542/peds.111.2.302 | quote = Thus, the shift in the evening bedtime across cohorts accounted for the substantial decrease in sleep duration in younger children between the 1970s and the 1990s. ... [A] more liberal parental attitude toward evening bedtime in the past decades is most likely responsible for the bedtime shift and for the decline of sleep duration... | pmid = 12563055 }}</ref>

== Functions ==
The multiple theories proposed to explain the function of sleep reflect the as yet incomplete understanding of the subject.

It is likely that sleep evolved to fulfill some primeval function, but has taken over multiple functions over time as organisms have evolved as with the larynx which today performs multiple functions such as controlling the passage of food and air, phonation for communicating, and social purposes.

Some of the many proposed functions of sleep are as follows.

===Restoration===
[[Wound healing]] has been shown to be affected by sleep. A study conducted by Gumustekin et al.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://websciences.org/cftemplate/NAPS/archives/indiv.cfm?ID=20044648 |title= Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats |accessdate= |author= |last= Gumustekin |first= K. |coauthors= Seven, B., Karabulut, N., Aktas, O., Gursan, N., Aslan, S., Keles, M., Varoglu, E., Dane S. |date= |year= 2004 |month= |format= Abstract |work= |publisher= Int J Neurosci 2004;114(11): 1433-42 |doi= |quote= }}</ref> in 2004 shows sleep deprivation hindering the [[healing]] of burns on rats.

It has also been shown that sleep deprivation affects the [[immune system]] and [[metabolism]]. In a study by Zager ''et al.'' in 2007,<ref>Zager, A., Andersen, M. L., Ruiz, F. S., Antunes, I. B., & Tufik, S. (2007). Effects of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats [Electronic version]. ''Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 293,'' R504-R509.</ref> rats were deprived of sleep for 24 hours. When compared with a control group, the sleep-deprived rats' blood tests indicated a 20% decrease in [[white blood cell]] count, a significant change in the immune system.

A study by Bonnet and Arand<ref>Bonnet, M. H. & Arand, D. L. (2003). Insomnia, metabolic rate and sleep restoration [Electronic version]. ''Journal of Internal Medicine, 254,'' 23–31.</ref> in 2003 indicates that sleep affects metabolism, is indeed a metabolic phase—anabolism. Comparing normal human sleepers and sleepers with ''sleep state misperception insomnia'', where patients complain of poor sleep but have normal sleep by electroencephalographic (EEG) criteria, the researchers found significantly greater metabolism values for the normal sleepers.

It has yet to be clearly proven that sleep ''duration'' affects [[somatic]] growth. One study by Jenni ''et al.''<ref>Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., Caflisch, J. A., & Largo, R. H. (2007). Sleep duration from ages 1 to 10 years: Variability and stability in comparison with growth [Electronic version]. ''Pediatrics, 120,'' e769-e776.</ref> in 2007 recorded growth, height and weight, as correlated to parent-reported time-in-bed in 305 children over a period of nine years (age 1–10). It was found that "the variation of sleep duration among children does not seem to have an effect on growth". It has been shown that sleep, more specifically slow-wave sleep (SWS), does affect [[growth hormone]] levels in adult men. During eight hours sleep, Van Cauter, Leproult, and Plat<ref>Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., & Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow-wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men [Electronic version]. ''Journal of the American Medical Association, 284,'' 861-868.</ref> found that the men with a high percentage of SWS (average 24%) also had high growth hormone secretion, while subjects with a low percentage of SWS (average 9%) had low growth hormone secretion.

There are multiple arguments supporting the restorative function of sleep. We are rested after sleeping and it is natural to assume that this is a basic purpose of sleep. The metabolic phase during sleep is anabolic; anabolic hormones such as growth hormones as mentioned above are secreted preferentially during sleep. The duration of sleep among species is, in general, inversely related to the animal size and directly related to [[basal metabolic rate]]. Rats with a very high basal metabolic rate sleep for up to 14 hours a day where as elephants and giraffes with lower BMRs sleep only 3–4 hours per day.

Energy conservation could as well have been accomplished by resting quiescent without shutting off the organism from the environment, potentially a dangerous situation. A sedentary non-sleeping animal is more likely to survive predators, while still preserving energy. Sleep therefore does something else other than conserving energy. Most interestingly, [[hibernation|hibernating]] animals that wake up from hibernation go into rebound sleep because of lack of sleep during the hibernation period. They are definitely well rested and are conserving energy during hibernation, but need sleep for something else.<ref name="pmid1945046">{{cite journal |author=Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM |title=Warming up for sleep? Ground squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation |journal=Neurosci. Lett. |volume=128 |issue=2 | pages = 581 |year=1991 |pmid=1945046 | doi = 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->|url=http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0304-3940(91)90276-Y}}</ref> Rats kept awake indefinitely develop skin lesions, [[hyperphagia]], loss of body mass, [[hypothermia]], and eventually [[septicemia]] and death.<ref> {{cite book |last= |first= |authorlink= |coauthors= |editor= |others= Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council |title= Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research |origdate= |origyear= |origmonth= |url= http://books.nap.edu/openbook.php?record_id=10732&page=121 |format= |accessdate= |accessyear= |accessmonth= |edition= |series= |volume= |date= |year= 2003 |month= |publisher= The National Academies Press |location= |isbn= 978-0-309-08903-6 |oclc= |doi= |id= |pages= pg 121 |chapter= |chapterurl= |quote= Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983).}} </ref>

===Anabolic/catabolic ===
[[Non-REM]] sleep may be an [[Anabolism|anabolic]] state marked by physiological processes of growth and rejuvenation of the organism's immune, nervous, muscular, and skeletal systems (with some exceptions). Wakefulness may perhaps be viewed as a cyclical, temporary, hyperactive [[catabolic]] state during which the organism acquires nourishment and reproduces.

===Ontogenesis===
According to the [[ontogeny|ontogenetic]] hypothesis of REM sleep, the activity occurring during neonatal REM sleep (or active sleep) seems to be particularly important to the developing organism (Marks et al., 1995). Studies investigating the effects of deprivation of active sleep have shown that deprivation early in life can result in behavioral problems, permanent sleep disruption, decreased brain mass (Mirmiran et al. 1983), and an abnormal amount of neuronal cell death (Morrissey, Duntley & Anch, 2004).

REM sleep appears to be important for development of the brain. REM sleep occupies the majority of time of sleep of infants, which spend most of their time sleeping. Among different species, the more immature the baby is born, the more time it spends in REM sleep. Proponents also suggest that REM-induced muscle inhibition in the presence of brain activation exists to allow for brain development by activating the synapses yet without any motor consequences which may get the infant in trouble. Additionally, REM deprivation results in developmental abnormalities later in life.

However, this does not explain why older adults still need REM sleep. Aquatic mammal infants do not have REM sleep in infancy <ref>{{cite web|title=Why do we Sleep?|url=http://www.slate.com/id/2162475/entry/2162477/|publisher=Slate.com|date=Mai 27, 2007|accessdate=2008-08-23|author=Amanda Schaffer}}</ref> REM sleep in those animals increases as they age.

===Memory processing===
Scientists have shown numerous ways in which sleep is related to [[memory]]. In a study conducted by Turner, Drummond, Salamat, and Brown<ref>Turner, T. H., Drummond, S. P. A., Salamat, J. S., & Brown, G. G. (2007). Effects of 42 hr sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory [Electronic version]. ''Neuropsychology, 21,'' 787-795.</ref> [[working memory]] was shown to be affected by sleep deprivation. Working memory is important because it keeps information active for further processing and supports higher-level [[cognitive functions]] such as [[decision making]], [[reasoning]], and [[episodic memory]]. Turner ''et al.'' allowed 18 women and 22 men to sleep only 26 minutes per night over a 4-day period. Subjects were given initial [[cognitive tests]] while well rested and then tested again twice a day during the 4 days of sleep deprivation. On the final test the average working memory span of the sleep deprived group had dropped by 38% in comparison to the control group.

Memory also seems to be affected differently by certain stages of sleep such as REM and [[slow-wave sleep]] (SWS). In one study cited in Born, Rasch, and Gais<ref>Born, J., Rasch, J., & Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember [Electronic version]. ''Neuroscientist, 12,'' 410.</ref> multiple groups of human subjects were used: wake control groups and sleep test groups. Sleep and wake groups were taught a task and then tested on it both on early and late nights, with the order of nights balanced across participants. When the subjects' brains were scanned during sleep, hypnograms revealed that SWS was the dominant sleep stage during the early night representing around 23% on average for sleep stage activity. The early night test group performed 16% better on the [[declarative memory]] test than the control group. During late night sleep, REM became the most active sleep stage at about 24%, and the late night test group performed 25% better on the [[procedural memory]] test than the control group. This indicates that procedural memory benefits from late REM-rich sleep whereas declarative memory benefits from early SWS-rich sleep.

Another study conducted by Datta<ref>Datta, S. (2000). Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity [Electronic version]. ''The Journal of Neuroscience, 20,'' 8607-8613.</ref> indirectly supports these results. The subjects chosen were 22 male rats. A box was constructed where a single rat could move freely from one end to the other. The bottom of the box was made of a steel grate. A light would shine in the box accompanied by a sound. After a 5 second delay an electrical shock would be applied. Once the shock commenced the rat could move to the other end of the box, ending the shock immediately. The rat could also use the 5-second delay to move to the other end of the box and avoid the shock entirely. The length of the shock never exceeded 5 seconds. This was repeated 30 times for half the rats. The other half, the control group, was placed in the same trial but the rats were shocked regardless of their reaction. After each of the training sessions the rat would be placed in a recording cage for 6 hours of polygraphic recordings. This process was repeated for 3 consecutive days. This study found that during the post-trial sleep recording session rats spent 25.47% more time in REM sleep after learning trials than after control trials. These trials support the results of the Born et al. study, indicating an obvious correlation between REM sleep and [[procedural knowledge]].

Another interesting observation of the Datta study is that the learning group spent 180% more time in SWS than did the control group during the post-trial sleep-recording session. This phenomenon is supported by a study performed by Kudrimoti, Barnes, and McNaughton.<ref>Kudrimoti, H. S., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: Effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics [Electronic version]. ''The Journal of Neuroscience, 19,'' 4090-4101.</ref> This study shows that after spatial exploration activity, patterns of [[Hippocampus|hippocampal]] place cells are reactivated during SWS following the experiment. In a study by Kudrimoti ''et al.'' seven rats were run through a linear track using rewards on either end. The rats would then be placed in the track for 30 minutes to allow them to adjust (PRE), then they ran the track with reward based training for 30 minutes (RUN), and then they were allowed to rest for 30 minutes. During each of these three periods [[Electroencephalograph|EEG]] data were collected for information on the rats' sleep stages. Kudrimoti ''et al.'' computed the mean firing rates of hippocampal place cells during pre-behavior SWS (PRE) and three 10-minute intervals in post-behavior SWS (POST) by averaging across 22 track-running sessions from seven rats. The results showed that 10 minutes after the trial RUN session there was a 12% increase in the mean firing rate of hippocampal place cells from the PRE level, however after 20 minutes the mean firing rate returned rapidly toward the PRE level. The elevated firing of hippocampal place cells during SWS after spatial exploration could explain why there were elevated levels of SWS sleep in Datta's study as it also dealt with a form of spatial exploration.

The different studies all suggest that there is a correlation between sleep and the many complex functions of memory. Harvard sleep researchers Saper and Stickgold<ref>"Nature, 10/05</ref> point out that an essential part of memory and learning consists of nerve cell [[dendrite]]s sending information to the cell body to be organized into new neuronal connections. This process demands that no external information is presented to these dendrites, and they suggest that this may be why it is during sleep that we solidify memories and organize knowledge.

{{further|[[Sleep and learning]], [[Sleep and creativity]]}}

=== Preservation ===
The "Preservation and Protection" theory holds that sleep serves an adaptive function. It protects the person during that portion of the 24-hour day in which being awake, and hence roaming around, would place the individual at greatest risk. Organisms do not require 24 hours to feed themselves and meet other necessities. From this perspective of adaptation, organisms are safer by staying out of harm's way where potentially they could be prey to other, stronger organisms. They sleep at times that maximize their safety, given their physical capacities and their habitats. (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Webb, 1982).

However, this theory fails to explain why the brain disengages from the external environment during normal sleep. Another argument against the theory is that sleep is not simply a passive consequence of removing the animal from the environment, but is a "drive": animals alter their behaviors in order to obtain sleep. Therefore, circadian regulation is more than sufficient to explain periods of activity and quiescence that are adaptive to an organism, but the more peculiar specializations of sleep probably serve different and unknown functions.

Moreover, the preservation theory does not explain why carnivores like lions, which are on top of the food chain, sleep the most. By the preservation logic, these top carnivores should not need any sleep at all. Preservation does not explain why aquatic mammals sleep while moving. Lethargy during these vulnerable hours would do the same, and will be more advantageous because the animal will be quiescent but still be able to respond to environmental challenges like predators etc. Sleep rebound that occurs after a sleepless night will be maladaptive, but still occurs for a reason. For example, a zebra falling asleep the day after it spent the sleeping time running from a lion is more and not less vulnerable to predation.

== Dreaming ==
{{main|Dream}}

Dreaming is the perception of sensory images and sounds during sleep, in a sequence which the sleeper/dreamer usually perceives more as an apparent participant than an observer. Dreaming is stimulated by the [[pons]] and mostly occurs during the [[REM phase of sleep]].

People have proposed many [[hypothesis|hypotheses]] about the functions of dreaming. [[Sigmund Freud]] postulated that dreams are the symbolic expression of frustrated desires that had been relegated to the [[subconscious]], and he used [[dream interpretation]] in the form of [[psychoanalysis]] to uncover these desires. Scientists have become skeptical about the Freudian interpretation, and place more emphasis on dreaming as a requirement for organization and consolidation of recent [[memory]] and experience. See Freud: [[The Interpretation of Dreams]]

[[Rosalind Cartwright]] stated that

{{quotation|One Function of dreams may be to restore our sense of competence.... it is also probable that in many times of stress, dreams have more work to do in resolving our problems and are thus more salient and memorable.<ref>{{cite news|url=http://www.sundayobserver.lk/2008/06/22/spe18.asp|title=Dream your dreams away|last=Nanayakkara|first=Anushka|work=[[The Observer]]|accessdate=2008-07-09}}</ref>|Rosalind Cartwright|[[The Sunday Observer]]}}

[[Allan Hobson|J. Allan Hobson's]] and [[Robert McCarley|Robert McCarley's]] [[activation synthesis theory]] proposes that dreams are caused by the [[random]] firing of [[neurons]] in the [[cerebral cortex]] during the REM period. According to the theory, the [[forebrain]] then creates a [[story]] in an attempt to reconcile and make sense of the nonsensical sensory information presented to it; hence the odd nature of many dreams.<ref>Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. (1977). The brain as a dream state of sex: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348.</ref>

== Effect of food and drink on sleep ==
{{Unreferencedsection|date=October 2008}}

=== Depressants ===
* [[Alcoholic beverage|Alcohol]]
Often people start drinking alcohol in order to get to sleep (alcohol is initially a sedative and will make you get to sleep faster). However, being addicted to alcohol can lead to disrupted sleep because alcohol has a rebound effect later in the night. As a result there is strong evidence linking alcoholism and insomnia.{{Fact|date=October 2008}}
* [[Barbiturate]]s
Barbiturates when taken cause drowsiness and have actions similar to ethanol (drinking alcohol).
* [[Melatonin]]
Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone that regulates sleepiness. It is made in the brain where tryptophan is converted into serotonin and then into melatonin, which is released at night by the [[pineal gland]] to induce and maintain sleep. Melatonin supplementation may be used as a sleep aid, both as a [[hypnotic]] and as a [[chronobiotic]] (see [[phase response curve]], PRC).
* [[Siesta]] and the "Post-Lunch Dip"
Many people have a temporary drop in alertness in early afternoon, commonly known as the ''post-lunch dip''. While a large meal, rich in carbohydrates, can make a person feel sleepy, the post-lunch dip is mostly an effect of the [[circadian rhythm|biological clock]]. People naturally feel most sleepy (have the greatest "drive for sleep") at two times of the day about 12 hours apart, for example at 2:00 AM and 2:00 PM. At those two times, the body clock "kicks in". At about 2 p.m. (14:00), it overrides the homeostatic build-up of sleep debt, allowing several more hours of wakefulness. At about 2 a.m. (02:00), with the daily sleep debt paid off, it "kicks in" again to ensure a few more hours of sleep.
* [[Tryptophan]]
The amino acid tryptophan is a building block of the protein found in foods. It contributes to sleepiness. Carbohydrates make tryptophan more available to the brain, which is why carbohydrate-heavy meals containing tryptophan tend to cause drowsiness. Tryptophan is a precursor to the neurotransmitter serotonin, which is a precursor to the neurohormone melatonin (see above).

=== Stimulants ===
* [[Amphetamines]]
Amphetamines ([[amphetamine]], [[dextroamphetamine]], [[methamphetamine]], etc) are often used to treat [[narcolepsy]] and [[ADHD]] disorders and are used recreationally, in which case they may be referred to as ''speed.'' Their most common effects are decreased hunger, anxiety, insomnia, stimulation, and increased alertness.
* [[Caffeine]]
Caffeine is a [[stimulant]] that works by slowing the action of the hormones in the brain that cause sleepiness, particularly by acting as a [[antagonist]] at [[adenosine]] receptors. Effective dosage is individual, in part dependent on prior usage. It can cause a rapid reduction in alertness as it wears off.
* [[Cocaine]] and [[Crack Cocaine]]
Studies on cocaine have shown its effects to be mediated through the circadian rhythm system.<ref>[http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12084940 Abarca C, Albrecht U, Spanagel R. ''Cocaine sensitization and reward are under the influence of circadian genes and rhythm.'' Department of Psychopharmacology, Central Institute of Mental Health, University of Heidelberg, J5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany.]</ref> This may be related to the onset of [[hypersomnia]] (over sleeping) in regard to "Cocaine-Induced Sleep Disorder"<ref>[http://www.mindsite.com/dsm_iv/primary_hypersomnia Primary hypersomnia: Diagnostic Features]</ref>
* [[Energy Drinks]]
The stimulating effects of energy drinks comes from stimulants such as caffeine, sugars, and [[essential amino acids]], and eventually will create a rapid reduction in alertness similar to that of caffeine.
* [[Methylenedioxymethamphetamine|MDMA]], including similar drugs like [[3,4-Methylenedioxyamphetamine|MDA]], [[MMDA (psychedelic)|MMDA]], or [[Methylenedioxymethcathinone|bk-MDMA]]
The class of drugs called [[empathogen-entactogen]]s keep users awake with intense euphoria. They are commonly known as ''ecstasy.''
* [[Methylphenidate]]
Commonly known by the brand names ''Ritalin'' & ''Concerta'', methylphenidate is similar in action to amphetamines and cocaine.

== Causes of difficulty in sleeping ==
There are a great many possible reasons for sleeping poorly. Following [[sleep hygiene|sleep hygienic]] principles may solve problems of physical or emotional discomfort.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.anxiety-and-depression-solutions.com/wellness_concerns/sleep/sleep_problem_causes.php |title= What Causes Sleep Difficulty? |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last=Little |first= Nan |date= 2007-01-01 |format= |work= |publisher= Insight Journal |quote= }}</ref> When pain, illness, drugs, or stress are the culprit, the cause must be treated. [[Sleep disorder]]s, including the [[sleep apnea]]s, [[narcolepsy]], primary insomnia, [[Nocturnal myoclonus|periodic limb movement disorder]] (PLMD), [[restless leg syndrome]] (RLS) and the [[circadian rhythm sleep disorder]]s, are treatable.

Elderly people may to some degree lose the ability to consolidate sleep. They need the same amount per day as they've always needed but may need to take some of their sleep as daytime naps.

== Anthropology of sleep ==

Recent research suggests that sleep patterns vary significantly across cultures.<ref>{{cite book | author = Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby | title=A comparative developmental ecology | chapter=6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep | chapterurl=http://webdrive.service.emory.edu/groups/research/lchb/PUBLICATIONS%20Worthman/PUBLICATIONS%20CMW%202002/Ecology%20of%20Human%20sleep.pdf | format=PDF | publisher=Emory University}}</ref><ref>[http://www.sciencenews.org/pages/sn_arc99/9_25_99/bob2.htm Slumber's Unexplored Landscape, Science News Online (9/25/99)]</ref> The most striking differences are between societies that have plentiful sources of artificial light and ones that do not. The primary difference appears to be that pre-light cultures have more broken up sleep patterns. For example, people might go to sleep far sooner after the sun sets, but then wake up several times throughout the night, punctuating their sleep with periods of wakefullness, perhaps lasting several hours. The boundaries between sleeping and waking are blurred in these societies. Some observers believe that sleep in these societies is most often split into two main periods, the first characterised primarily by deep sleep and the second by REM sleep. This [[segmented sleep]] has led to expressions such as "first sleep", "watch" and "second sleep" which appear in literature from [[pre-industrial]] societies all over the world.

Some societies display a fragmented sleep pattern in which people sleep at all times of the day and night for shorter periods. For example, many Mediterranean societies have a ''[[siesta]]'', in which people sleep for a period in the [[afternoon]]. In many [[nomadic]] or [[hunter-gatherer]] societies people will sleep off and on throughout the day or night depending on what is happening. <ref>{{cite book | author = Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby | title=A comparative developmental ecology | chapter=6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep | chapterurl=http://webdrive.service.emory.edu/groups/research/lchb/PUBLICATIONS%20Worthman/PUBLICATIONS%20CMW%202002/Ecology%20of%20Human%20sleep.pdf | format=PDF | publisher=Emory University}}</ref>

Plentiful [[artificial light]] has been available in the industrialised west since at least the mid-nineteenth century, and sleep patterns have changed significantly everywhere that lighting has been introduced. In general, people sleep in a more concentrated burst through the night, going to sleep much later, although this is not always true.

In some societies people generally sleep with at least one other person, often many, or with animals. In others people rarely sleep with anyone but a most intimate relation such as a spouse. In almost all societies sleeping partners are strongly regulated by social standards. For example, people might only sleep with their [[immediate family]], [[extended family]], spouses, with their children, with children of a certain age, children of specific gender, peers of a certain gender, friends, peers of equal social rank, or with no one at all. Sleep may be an actively social time depending on the sleep groupings, with no constraints on noise or activity. <ref>{{cite book | author = Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby | title=A comparative developmental ecology | chapter=6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep | chapterurl=http://webdrive.service.emory.edu/groups/research/lchb/PUBLICATIONS%20Worthman/PUBLICATIONS%20CMW%202002/Ecology%20of%20Human%20sleep.pdf | format=PDF | publisher=Emory University}}</ref>

People sleep in a variety of locations. Some sleep directly on the ground, others on a skin or [[blanket]], others sleep on platforms or [[bed]]s. Some sleep with blankets, some with [[pillow]]s, some with simple head rests, some with no head support. These choices are shaped by a variety of factors such as climate, protection from predators, housing type, technology, and the incidence of [[pest]]s. <ref>{{cite book | author = Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby | title=A comparative developmental ecology | chapter=6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep | chapterurl=http://webdrive.service.emory.edu/groups/research/lchb/PUBLICATIONS%20Worthman/PUBLICATIONS%20CMW%202002/Ecology%20of%20Human%20sleep.pdf | format=PDF | publisher=Emory University}}</ref>

== Sleep in non-humans ==
[[Image:Japanese Macaques sleeping.JPG|thumb|right|Sleeping [[Japanese Macaque]]s.]]
{{main|Sleep (non-human)}}

Horses and other herbivorous ungulates can sleep while standing, but must necessarily lie down for REM sleep (which causes muscular atony) for short periods - giraffes, for example, only need to lie down for REM sleep for a few minutes at a time. Bats sleep while hanging upside down. Some aquatic mammals and some birds can sleep with one half of the brain, while the other half is awake, so called [[unihemispheric slow-wave sleep]].<ref>{{cite journal | author = Mukhametova LM | coauthors = Supina AY, Polyakovaa IG | title = Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins | journal = Brain Research | volume = 134 | issue = 3 | pages = 581–584 | publisher = |date=1977-10-14 | url = | doi = 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90835-6 | pmid = 902119 | accessdate = }}</ref> Birds and mammals have cycles of non-REM and REM sleep as described above for humans, though birds’ cycles are much shorter and they do not lose muscle tone (go limp) to the same extent that most mammals do.

Many animals sleep, but neurological sleep states are difficult to define in lower order animals. In these animals, sleep is defined using behavioral characteristics such as minimal movement, postures typical for the species and reduced responsiveness to external stimulation. Sleep is quickly reversible, as opposed to hibernation or [[coma]], and sleep deprivation is followed by longer and/or deeper sleep. Herbivores, who require a long waking period to gather and consume their diet, typically sleep less each day than similarly sized carnivores who might well consume several days supply of meat in a sitting.

Many species of mammals sleep for a large proportion of each 24-hour period when they are very young.<ref>{{cite web |url= http://www.madsci.org/posts/archives/2000-08/965504574.Zo.r.html |title= Re: Are there animals who don't sleep or that sleep very little? |accessdate= 2008-01-25 |last= Faraco |first= Juliette |coauthors= |date= 2000-08-01 |format= |work= MadSci Network: Zoology |publisher= |pages= |quote= }}</ref> However, [[killer whale]]s and some [[dolphin]]s do not sleep during the first month of life.<ref>[http://www.livescience.com/animals/050629_sleepless_sea.html Insomnia Mania: Newborn Mammals Don't Sleep for a Month | LiveScience<!-- Bot generated title -->]</ref> Such differences may be explained by the ability of land mammal newborns to be easily protected by parents while sleeping, while marine animals must, even while very young, be more continuously vigilant for predators.

William C Dement in his book "The Promise of Sleep". states that the dolphin, originally a land mammal that has returned to the sea,
maintains a number of terrestrial traits including bearing their offspring alive and, unlike fish, breathing air. When terrestrial mammals breathe, they do so through an involuntary process similar to the one that causes our hearts to beat continuously. In dolphins the breathing process is under voluntary control throughout the day --- a process that, seemingly, would preclude sleep. In order to accomplish what seems to be an impossible task, dolphins allow one half of their brain to go to sleep while the other half remains awake. This is accomplished in two-hour cycles where one half of the brain is awake while the other half sleeps until the dolphin's day's sleep need has been fulfilled.

==See also==
{{commonscat}}
===Common sleeping positions, practices, and rituals===
* [[Co-sleeping]]
* [[Hypnosis]]
* [[Meditation]]
* [[Neutral spine]]
* [[Sleep hygiene]]
* [[Yoga Nidra]]

=== Other ===
* [[Alarm clock]]
* [[Dream world (plot device)]]
* [[Microsleep]]
* [[Morvan's syndrome]]
* [[Sudden infant death syndrome]]

== References ==
{{Reflist|2}}

== Further reading ==
* {{cite book | author=Bar-Yam, Yaneer | authorlink = Yaneer Bar-Yam | title = Dynamics of Complex Systems | chapter=Chapter 3 | chapterurl=http://necsi.org/publications/dcs/Bar-YamChap3.pdf | format=PDF | year = 2003}}
* {{cite journal | author = Foldvary-Schaefer N, Grigg-Damberger M | title = Sleep and epilepsy: what we know, don't know, and need to know | journal = J Clin Neurophysiol | volume = 23 | issue = 1 | pages = 4–20 | year = 2006 | month=February | pmid = 16514348 | doi = 10.1097/01.wnp.0000206877.90232.cb}}
* {{cite journal | author = Gilmartin G, Thomas R | title = Mechanisms of arousal from sleep and their consequences | journal = Curr Opin Pulm Med | volume = 10 | issue = 6 | pages = 468–74 | year = 2004 | month=November | pmid = 15510052 | doi = 10.1097/01.mcp.0000143690.94442.b3 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}} [Review]
* {{cite journal | author = Gottlieb D, Punjabi N, Newman A, Resnick H, Redline S, Baldwin C, Nieto F | title = Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance | journal = Arch Intern Med | volume = 165 | issue = 8 | pages = 863–7 | year = 2005 | month=Apr 25 | pmid = 15851636 | doi = 10.1001/archinte.165.8.863 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Legramante J, Galante A | title = Sleep and hypertension: a challenge for the autonomic regulation of the cardiovascular system | journal = Circulation | volume = 112 | issue = 6 | pages = 786–8 | year = 2005 | month=Aug 9 | pmid = 16087808 | doi= 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.555714}} [Editorial]
* Feinberg I. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age [http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=PubMed&cmd=Search&defaultField=Title+Word&term=J+Psychiatr+Res%5Bjour%5D+AND+10%5Bvolume%5D+AND+283%5Bpage%5D+AND+1974%5Bpdat%5D+AND+Feinberg+I%5Bauth%5D J Psychiatr Res. 1974;10:283–306.] [review]
* Dement, William C., M.D., Ph.D. The Promise of Sleep. Delacorte Press, Random House Inc., New York, 1999.
* Tamar Shochat and Sonia Ancoli - [http://sleep.health.am/sleep/more/specific-clinical-patterns-in-aging/ Specific Clinical Patterns in Aging] - Sleep and Sleep Disorders [website]
* Zepelin H. Normal age related changes in sleep. In: Chase M, Weitzman ED, eds. Sleep Disorders: Basic and Clinical Research. New York: SP Medical; 1983:431–434.
* {{cite journal | author = Morrissey M, Duntley S, Anch A, Nonneman R | title = Active sleep and its role in the prevention of apoptosis in the developing brain | journal = Med Hypotheses | volume = 62 | issue = 6 | pages = 876–9 | year = 2004 | pmid = 15142640 | doi = 10.1016/j.mehy.2004.01.014 <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Marks G, Shaffery J, Oksenberg A, Speciale S, Roffwarg H | title = A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation | journal = Behav Brain Res | volume = 69 | issue = 1–2 | pages = 1–11 | year = 1995 | month = Jul-Aug | pmid = 7546299 | doi = 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-O <!--Retrieved from CrossRef by DOI bot-->}}
* {{cite journal | author = Mirmiran M, Scholtens J, van de Poll N, Uylings H, van der Gugten J, Boer G | title = Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat | journal = Brain Res | volume = 283 | issue = 2–3 | pages = 277–86 | year = 1983 | month = April | pmid = 6850353}}
* {{cite journal | author=Zhang, J. | title = [Memory process and the function of sleep] | journal=Journal of Theoretics | volume=6 | issue = 6 | year = 2004 | month = December | url=http://www.journaloftheoretics.com/Articles/6-6/Zhang.pdf | format=PDF}}

==External links==
{{wiktionary}}
* [http://www.aasmnet.org American Academy of Sleep Medicine]
*[http://www.sleepassociation.org American Sleep Association]
* [http://healthysleep.med.harvard.edu/ Healthy Sleep] from the Division of Sleep Medicine at [[Harvard Medical School]] and [[WGBH]] Educational Foundation
* [http://biology.plosjournals.org/perlserv/?request=get-document&doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.0060216&ct=1 Is Sleep Essential?] from the [[Public Library of Science]] Biology
* [http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/about/ncsdr/index.htm National Center on Sleep Disorders Research]
* [http://www.sleepfoundation.org National Sleep Foundation]
* [http://www.sleepresearchsociety.org Sleep Research Society]
* [http://www.sleepreviewmag.com Sleep Review]

{{SleepSeries2}}

[[Category:Sleep]]
[[Category:Unsolved problems in neuroscience]]

[[ar:نوم]]
[[be:Сон]]
[[be-x-old:Сон]]
[[bs:Spavanje]]
[[bg:Сън]]
[[ca:Son]]
[[cs:Spánek]]
[[cy:Cwsg]]
[[da:Søvn]]
[[de:Schlaf]]
[[et:Uni]]
[[el:Ύπνος]]
[[es:Sueño]]
[[eo:Dormo]]
[[eu:Lo]]
[[fa:خواب]]
[[fr:Sommeil]]
[[gd:Cadal]]
[[gl:Sono]]
[[gan:睏覺]]
[[ko:잠]]
[[hi:निद्रा]]
[[hr:Spavanje]]
[[id:Tidur]]
[[is:Svefn]]
[[it:Sonno]]
[[he:שינה]]
[[lv:Miegs]]
[[lt:Miegas]]
[[hu:Alvás]]
[[ml:ഉറക്കം]]
[[mr:झोप]]
[[ms:Tidur]]
[[nl:Slaap (rust)]]
[[new:न्ह्यः]]
[[ja:睡眠]]
[[no:Søvn]]
[[nn:Søvn]]
[[uz:Uyqu]]
[[pl:Sen]]
[[pt:Sono]]
[[ro:Somn]]
[[qu:Puñuy]]
[[ru:Сон]]
[[sq:Gjumi]]
[[scn:Sonnu]]
[[simple:Sleep]]
[[sk:Spánok]]
[[sl:Uspavalo]]
[[sh:Spavanje]]
[[su:Saré]]
[[fi:Nukkuminen]]
[[sv:Sömn]]
[[tl:Tulog]]
[[tg:Хоб]]
[[tr:Uyku]]
[[uk:Сон (значення)]]
[[ur:نیند]]
[[yi:שלאף]]
[[zh-yue:睏覺]]
[[zh:睡眠]]

Revision as of 11:02, 3 January 2009

Sleepy men in Tehran, Iran

Sleep is a natural state of bodily rest observed throughout the animal kingdom. It is common to all mammals and birds, and is also seen in many reptiles, amphibians and fish. In humans, other mammals, and a substantial majority of other animals which have been studied — such as fish, birds, ants, and fruit-flies — regular sleep is essential for survival. A widely publicized 2003 study performed at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.[1] However, the purposes of sleep are only partly clear and are the subject of intense research.[2]

Physiology

In mammals and birds, sleep is divided into two broad types: Rapid Eye Movement (REM) and Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM) or "Non-REM" sleep. Each type has a distinct set of associated physiological, neurological and psychological features.

Sleep proceeds in cycles of REM and the three stages of NREM, the order normally being:

stages N1 -> N2 -> N3 -> N2 -> REM.
Sleep cycles through the night with deep sleep early on and more REM, marked in red, toward morning

(Until recently, NREM was considered to include four stages and the cycle was N1 -> N2 -> N3 -> N4 -> N3 -> N2 -> REM; this system is still seen in reference works and diagrams.)

In humans each sleep cycle lasts on average 90 to 110 minutes,[3] with a greater amount of stage 3 (N3) early in the night and more REM later in the night. Each phase may have a distinct physiological function. Drugs such as sleeping pills and alcoholic beverages can suppress certain stages of sleep, leading to sleep deprivation. This can result in sleep that exhibits loss of consciousness but does not fulfill its physiological functions.

Allan Rechtschaffen and Anthony Kales originally outlined the criteria for identifying the stages of sleep in 1968. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) updated the staging rules in 2007.

Stage 4 Sleep. EEG highlighted by red box.
REM sleep. EEG highlighted by red box. Eye movements highlighted by red line.

Stages of sleep

Sleep stages and other characteristics of sleep can be measured by polysomnography in a sleep laboratory using among other tools electroencephalography (EEG) for brain waves, electrooculography (EOG) for eye movements and electromyography (EMG) for activity of skeletal muscles.

Criteria for REM sleep include not only rapid eye movements but also rapid low voltage EEG, commonly called brain waves. In mammals, at least, low muscle tone is also seen, often called paralysis. Most memorable dreaming occurs in this stage. REM sleep accounts for 20–25% of total sleep time in normal human adults; NREM (non-REM) accounting for the rest. In NREM sleep, there is relatively little dreaming.

Non-REM encompasses three stages; stage 1 (N1), stage 2 (N2), and stage 3 (N3); N3 being referred to as 'deep sleep' or slow-wave sleep, SWS. Sleep stages are differentiated by a combination of brain waves, eyes movements, and skeletal muscle activity. In non-REM sleep there may be limb movements, and parasomnias such as sleepwalking may occur.

NREM consists of three stages according to the 2007 AASM standards:

  • During Stage N1 the brain transitions from alpha waves (having a frequency of 8 to 13 Hz, common to people who are awake) to theta waves (with a frequency of 4 to 7 Hz). This stage is sometimes referred to as somnolence, or "drowsy sleep". Associated with the onset of sleep during N1 may be sudden twitches and hypnic jerks also known as positive myoclonus. Some people may also experience hypnagogic hallucinations during this stage, which can be troublesome to them. During N1 the subject loses some muscle tone and most conscious awareness of the external environment.
  • Stage N2, is characterized by "sleep spindles" (12 to 16 Hz) and "K-complexes." During this stage, muscular activity as measured by EMG decreases and conscious awareness of the external environment disappears. This stage occupies 45 to 55% of total sleep.
  • Stage N3, deep or slow-wave sleep (SWS), is characterized by delta waves, (0.5 to 4 Hz), also called delta rhythms. This is the stage in which night terrors, bedwetting, sleepwalking and sleep-talking occur. In the previous system, the difference between stages 3 and 4 (now combined as 3) was that delta waves made up less than 50% of the total wave-patterns in stage 3, while they were more than 50% in stage 4.
  • A recent ruling by the AASM discontinued stage 4 sleep (N4) and left only stage N3 to describe delta sleep.[4]

Both REM sleep and SWS sleep (previously stages 3 and 4, now combined as stage 3) are homeostatically driven; that is, people and most animals selectively deprived of one of these stages will rebound once uninhibited sleep is allowed. This finding suggests that both types of sleep are essential.

Sleep timing

Sleep timing is controlled by the circadian clock, by homeostasis and in humans, within certain bounds, by willed behavior. The circadian clock, an inner time-keeping, temperature-fluctuating, enzyme-controlling device, works in tandem with adenosine, a neurotransmitter which inhibits many of the bodily processes that are associated with wakefulness. Adenosine is created over the course of the day; high levels of adenosine lead to sleepiness. In diurnal animals, sleepiness occurs as the circadian element causes the release of the hormone melatonin and a gradual decrease in core body temperature. The timing is affected by one's chronotype. It is the circadian rhythm which determines the ideal timing of a correctly structured and restorative sleep episode.[5]

Homeostatic sleep propensity, the need for sleep as a function of the amount of time elapsed since the last adequate sleep episode, is also important and must be balanced against the circadian element for satisfactory sleep. Along with corresponding messages from the circadian clock, this tells the body it needs to sleep.[6] Sleep offset, awakening, is primarily determined by circadian rhythm. A normal person who regularly awakens at an early hour will generally not be able to sleep much later than the person's normal waking time, even if moderately sleep deprived.

Optimal amount in humans

Adults

The optimal amount of sleep is not a meaningful concept unless the timing of that sleep is seen in relation to an individual's circadian rhythms. A person's major sleep episode is relatively inefficient and inadequate when it occurs at the "wrong" time of day. The timing is correct when the following two circadian markers occur after the middle of the sleep episode but before awakening:[7]

  • maximum concentration of the hormone melatonin, and
  • minimum core body temperature.

The National Sleep Foundation in the United States maintains that seven to nine hours of sleep for adult humans is optimal and that sufficient sleep benefits alertness, memory and problem solving, and overall health, as well as reducing the risk of accidents.[8] A widely publicized 2003 study[9] performed at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine demonstrated that cognitive performance declines with fewer than eight hours of sleep.

However, a University of California, San Diego psychiatry study of more than one million adults found that people who live the longest self-report sleeping for six to seven hours each night.[10] Another study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women showed similar results.[11] Other studies show that "sleeping more than 7 to 8 hours per day has been consistently associated with increased mortality", though this study suggests the cause is probably other factors such as depression and socio-economic status which would correlate statistically. [12] It has been suggested that the correlation between lower sleep hours and reduced morbidity only occurs with those who wake after less sleep naturally, rather than those who use an alarm.

A Kutchi woman sleeping in Nirona village

Causal links are currently speculative: the available data may only reflect comorbid depression, socioeconomic status, or even alcohol use, for example.[13] These studies cannot be used to determine optimal sleep habits, only correlation — and an empirically observed correlation is a necessary but not sufficient condition for causality. A need for nine or ten hours of sleep a day, or only five to six, may or may not have the same cause as the shortened life span. In other words, long or short sleep duration itself has not been shown to be a cause of early death.

Researchers from the University of Warwick and University College London have found that lack of sleep can more than double the risk of death from cardiovascular disease, but that too much sleep can also double the risk of death.[14][15] Professor Francesco Cappuccio said: “Short sleep has been shown to be a risk factor for weight gain, hypertension and Type 2 diabetes sometimes leading to mortality but in contrast to the short sleep-mortality association it appears that no potential mechanisms by which long sleep could be associated with increased mortality have yet been investigated. Some candidate causes for this include depression, low socioeconomic status and cancer-related fatigue. [...] In terms of prevention, our findings indicate that consistently sleeping around 7 hours per night is optimal for health and a sustained reduction may predispose to ill-health.”

Furthermore, sleep difficulties are closely associated with psychiatric disorders such as depression, alcoholism and bipolar disorder. Up to 90% of patients with depression are found to have sleep difficulties.[citation needed]

Hours by age

A child sleeping

Children need a greater amount of sleep per day than adults to develop and function properly: up to 18 hours for newborn babies, with a declining rate as a child ages.[8][6] A newborn baby spends almost half of its sleep time in REM-sleep. By the age of five or so, only a bit over two hours are spent in REM.[16]

Age and condition Average amount of sleep per day
Newborn up to 18 hours
1–12 months 14–18 hours
1–3 years 12–15 hours
3–5 years 11–13 hours
5–12 years 9–11 hours
Adolescents 9-10
Adults, including elderly 7–8 (+) hours
Pregnant women 8 (+) hours

Sleep debt

Sleep debt is the effect of not getting quite enough rest and sleep; a large debt causes mental, emotional and physical fatigue. It is unclear why a lack of sleep causes irritability; however theories are emerging that suggest if the body produces insufficient cortisol during stage 3 and 4 sleep it can have negative effects on our alertness and emotions during the day.[17][citation needed]

Scientists do not agree on how much sleep debt it is possible to accumulate, whether it is accumulated against an individual's average sleep or some other benchmark, nor on whether the prevalence of sleep debt among adults has changed appreciably in the industrialized world in recent decades. It is likely that children are sleeping less than previously in western societies.[18]

Functions

The multiple theories proposed to explain the function of sleep reflect the as yet incomplete understanding of the subject.

It is likely that sleep evolved to fulfill some primeval function, but has taken over multiple functions over time as organisms have evolved as with the larynx which today performs multiple functions such as controlling the passage of food and air, phonation for communicating, and social purposes.

Some of the many proposed functions of sleep are as follows.

Restoration

Wound healing has been shown to be affected by sleep. A study conducted by Gumustekin et al.[19] in 2004 shows sleep deprivation hindering the healing of burns on rats.

It has also been shown that sleep deprivation affects the immune system and metabolism. In a study by Zager et al. in 2007,[20] rats were deprived of sleep for 24 hours. When compared with a control group, the sleep-deprived rats' blood tests indicated a 20% decrease in white blood cell count, a significant change in the immune system.

A study by Bonnet and Arand[21] in 2003 indicates that sleep affects metabolism, is indeed a metabolic phase—anabolism. Comparing normal human sleepers and sleepers with sleep state misperception insomnia, where patients complain of poor sleep but have normal sleep by electroencephalographic (EEG) criteria, the researchers found significantly greater metabolism values for the normal sleepers.

It has yet to be clearly proven that sleep duration affects somatic growth. One study by Jenni et al.[22] in 2007 recorded growth, height and weight, as correlated to parent-reported time-in-bed in 305 children over a period of nine years (age 1–10). It was found that "the variation of sleep duration among children does not seem to have an effect on growth". It has been shown that sleep, more specifically slow-wave sleep (SWS), does affect growth hormone levels in adult men. During eight hours sleep, Van Cauter, Leproult, and Plat[23] found that the men with a high percentage of SWS (average 24%) also had high growth hormone secretion, while subjects with a low percentage of SWS (average 9%) had low growth hormone secretion.

There are multiple arguments supporting the restorative function of sleep. We are rested after sleeping and it is natural to assume that this is a basic purpose of sleep. The metabolic phase during sleep is anabolic; anabolic hormones such as growth hormones as mentioned above are secreted preferentially during sleep. The duration of sleep among species is, in general, inversely related to the animal size and directly related to basal metabolic rate. Rats with a very high basal metabolic rate sleep for up to 14 hours a day where as elephants and giraffes with lower BMRs sleep only 3–4 hours per day.

Energy conservation could as well have been accomplished by resting quiescent without shutting off the organism from the environment, potentially a dangerous situation. A sedentary non-sleeping animal is more likely to survive predators, while still preserving energy. Sleep therefore does something else other than conserving energy. Most interestingly, hibernating animals that wake up from hibernation go into rebound sleep because of lack of sleep during the hibernation period. They are definitely well rested and are conserving energy during hibernation, but need sleep for something else.[24] Rats kept awake indefinitely develop skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death.[25]

Anabolic/catabolic

Non-REM sleep may be an anabolic state marked by physiological processes of growth and rejuvenation of the organism's immune, nervous, muscular, and skeletal systems (with some exceptions). Wakefulness may perhaps be viewed as a cyclical, temporary, hyperactive catabolic state during which the organism acquires nourishment and reproduces.

Ontogenesis

According to the ontogenetic hypothesis of REM sleep, the activity occurring during neonatal REM sleep (or active sleep) seems to be particularly important to the developing organism (Marks et al., 1995). Studies investigating the effects of deprivation of active sleep have shown that deprivation early in life can result in behavioral problems, permanent sleep disruption, decreased brain mass (Mirmiran et al. 1983), and an abnormal amount of neuronal cell death (Morrissey, Duntley & Anch, 2004).

REM sleep appears to be important for development of the brain. REM sleep occupies the majority of time of sleep of infants, which spend most of their time sleeping. Among different species, the more immature the baby is born, the more time it spends in REM sleep. Proponents also suggest that REM-induced muscle inhibition in the presence of brain activation exists to allow for brain development by activating the synapses yet without any motor consequences which may get the infant in trouble. Additionally, REM deprivation results in developmental abnormalities later in life.

However, this does not explain why older adults still need REM sleep. Aquatic mammal infants do not have REM sleep in infancy [26] REM sleep in those animals increases as they age.

Memory processing

Scientists have shown numerous ways in which sleep is related to memory. In a study conducted by Turner, Drummond, Salamat, and Brown[27] working memory was shown to be affected by sleep deprivation. Working memory is important because it keeps information active for further processing and supports higher-level cognitive functions such as decision making, reasoning, and episodic memory. Turner et al. allowed 18 women and 22 men to sleep only 26 minutes per night over a 4-day period. Subjects were given initial cognitive tests while well rested and then tested again twice a day during the 4 days of sleep deprivation. On the final test the average working memory span of the sleep deprived group had dropped by 38% in comparison to the control group.

Memory also seems to be affected differently by certain stages of sleep such as REM and slow-wave sleep (SWS). In one study cited in Born, Rasch, and Gais[28] multiple groups of human subjects were used: wake control groups and sleep test groups. Sleep and wake groups were taught a task and then tested on it both on early and late nights, with the order of nights balanced across participants. When the subjects' brains were scanned during sleep, hypnograms revealed that SWS was the dominant sleep stage during the early night representing around 23% on average for sleep stage activity. The early night test group performed 16% better on the declarative memory test than the control group. During late night sleep, REM became the most active sleep stage at about 24%, and the late night test group performed 25% better on the procedural memory test than the control group. This indicates that procedural memory benefits from late REM-rich sleep whereas declarative memory benefits from early SWS-rich sleep.

Another study conducted by Datta[29] indirectly supports these results. The subjects chosen were 22 male rats. A box was constructed where a single rat could move freely from one end to the other. The bottom of the box was made of a steel grate. A light would shine in the box accompanied by a sound. After a 5 second delay an electrical shock would be applied. Once the shock commenced the rat could move to the other end of the box, ending the shock immediately. The rat could also use the 5-second delay to move to the other end of the box and avoid the shock entirely. The length of the shock never exceeded 5 seconds. This was repeated 30 times for half the rats. The other half, the control group, was placed in the same trial but the rats were shocked regardless of their reaction. After each of the training sessions the rat would be placed in a recording cage for 6 hours of polygraphic recordings. This process was repeated for 3 consecutive days. This study found that during the post-trial sleep recording session rats spent 25.47% more time in REM sleep after learning trials than after control trials. These trials support the results of the Born et al. study, indicating an obvious correlation between REM sleep and procedural knowledge.

Another interesting observation of the Datta study is that the learning group spent 180% more time in SWS than did the control group during the post-trial sleep-recording session. This phenomenon is supported by a study performed by Kudrimoti, Barnes, and McNaughton.[30] This study shows that after spatial exploration activity, patterns of hippocampal place cells are reactivated during SWS following the experiment. In a study by Kudrimoti et al. seven rats were run through a linear track using rewards on either end. The rats would then be placed in the track for 30 minutes to allow them to adjust (PRE), then they ran the track with reward based training for 30 minutes (RUN), and then they were allowed to rest for 30 minutes. During each of these three periods EEG data were collected for information on the rats' sleep stages. Kudrimoti et al. computed the mean firing rates of hippocampal place cells during pre-behavior SWS (PRE) and three 10-minute intervals in post-behavior SWS (POST) by averaging across 22 track-running sessions from seven rats. The results showed that 10 minutes after the trial RUN session there was a 12% increase in the mean firing rate of hippocampal place cells from the PRE level, however after 20 minutes the mean firing rate returned rapidly toward the PRE level. The elevated firing of hippocampal place cells during SWS after spatial exploration could explain why there were elevated levels of SWS sleep in Datta's study as it also dealt with a form of spatial exploration.

The different studies all suggest that there is a correlation between sleep and the many complex functions of memory. Harvard sleep researchers Saper and Stickgold[31] point out that an essential part of memory and learning consists of nerve cell dendrites sending information to the cell body to be organized into new neuronal connections. This process demands that no external information is presented to these dendrites, and they suggest that this may be why it is during sleep that we solidify memories and organize knowledge.

Preservation

The "Preservation and Protection" theory holds that sleep serves an adaptive function. It protects the person during that portion of the 24-hour day in which being awake, and hence roaming around, would place the individual at greatest risk. Organisms do not require 24 hours to feed themselves and meet other necessities. From this perspective of adaptation, organisms are safer by staying out of harm's way where potentially they could be prey to other, stronger organisms. They sleep at times that maximize their safety, given their physical capacities and their habitats. (Allison & Cicchetti, 1976; Webb, 1982).

However, this theory fails to explain why the brain disengages from the external environment during normal sleep. Another argument against the theory is that sleep is not simply a passive consequence of removing the animal from the environment, but is a "drive": animals alter their behaviors in order to obtain sleep. Therefore, circadian regulation is more than sufficient to explain periods of activity and quiescence that are adaptive to an organism, but the more peculiar specializations of sleep probably serve different and unknown functions.

Moreover, the preservation theory does not explain why carnivores like lions, which are on top of the food chain, sleep the most. By the preservation logic, these top carnivores should not need any sleep at all. Preservation does not explain why aquatic mammals sleep while moving. Lethargy during these vulnerable hours would do the same, and will be more advantageous because the animal will be quiescent but still be able to respond to environmental challenges like predators etc. Sleep rebound that occurs after a sleepless night will be maladaptive, but still occurs for a reason. For example, a zebra falling asleep the day after it spent the sleeping time running from a lion is more and not less vulnerable to predation.

Dreaming

Dreaming is the perception of sensory images and sounds during sleep, in a sequence which the sleeper/dreamer usually perceives more as an apparent participant than an observer. Dreaming is stimulated by the pons and mostly occurs during the REM phase of sleep.

People have proposed many hypotheses about the functions of dreaming. Sigmund Freud postulated that dreams are the symbolic expression of frustrated desires that had been relegated to the subconscious, and he used dream interpretation in the form of psychoanalysis to uncover these desires. Scientists have become skeptical about the Freudian interpretation, and place more emphasis on dreaming as a requirement for organization and consolidation of recent memory and experience. See Freud: The Interpretation of Dreams

Rosalind Cartwright stated that

One Function of dreams may be to restore our sense of competence.... it is also probable that in many times of stress, dreams have more work to do in resolving our problems and are thus more salient and memorable.[32]

— Rosalind Cartwright, The Sunday Observer

J. Allan Hobson's and Robert McCarley's activation synthesis theory proposes that dreams are caused by the random firing of neurons in the cerebral cortex during the REM period. According to the theory, the forebrain then creates a story in an attempt to reconcile and make sense of the nonsensical sensory information presented to it; hence the odd nature of many dreams.[33]

Effect of food and drink on sleep

Depressants

Often people start drinking alcohol in order to get to sleep (alcohol is initially a sedative and will make you get to sleep faster). However, being addicted to alcohol can lead to disrupted sleep because alcohol has a rebound effect later in the night. As a result there is strong evidence linking alcoholism and insomnia.[citation needed]

Barbiturates when taken cause drowsiness and have actions similar to ethanol (drinking alcohol).

Melatonin is a naturally occurring hormone that regulates sleepiness. It is made in the brain where tryptophan is converted into serotonin and then into melatonin, which is released at night by the pineal gland to induce and maintain sleep. Melatonin supplementation may be used as a sleep aid, both as a hypnotic and as a chronobiotic (see phase response curve, PRC).

  • Siesta and the "Post-Lunch Dip"

Many people have a temporary drop in alertness in early afternoon, commonly known as the post-lunch dip. While a large meal, rich in carbohydrates, can make a person feel sleepy, the post-lunch dip is mostly an effect of the biological clock. People naturally feel most sleepy (have the greatest "drive for sleep") at two times of the day about 12 hours apart, for example at 2:00 AM and 2:00 PM. At those two times, the body clock "kicks in". At about 2 p.m. (14:00), it overrides the homeostatic build-up of sleep debt, allowing several more hours of wakefulness. At about 2 a.m. (02:00), with the daily sleep debt paid off, it "kicks in" again to ensure a few more hours of sleep.

The amino acid tryptophan is a building block of the protein found in foods. It contributes to sleepiness. Carbohydrates make tryptophan more available to the brain, which is why carbohydrate-heavy meals containing tryptophan tend to cause drowsiness. Tryptophan is a precursor to the neurotransmitter serotonin, which is a precursor to the neurohormone melatonin (see above).

Stimulants

Amphetamines (amphetamine, dextroamphetamine, methamphetamine, etc) are often used to treat narcolepsy and ADHD disorders and are used recreationally, in which case they may be referred to as speed. Their most common effects are decreased hunger, anxiety, insomnia, stimulation, and increased alertness.

Caffeine is a stimulant that works by slowing the action of the hormones in the brain that cause sleepiness, particularly by acting as a antagonist at adenosine receptors. Effective dosage is individual, in part dependent on prior usage. It can cause a rapid reduction in alertness as it wears off.

Studies on cocaine have shown its effects to be mediated through the circadian rhythm system.[34] This may be related to the onset of hypersomnia (over sleeping) in regard to "Cocaine-Induced Sleep Disorder"[35]

The stimulating effects of energy drinks comes from stimulants such as caffeine, sugars, and essential amino acids, and eventually will create a rapid reduction in alertness similar to that of caffeine.

The class of drugs called empathogen-entactogens keep users awake with intense euphoria. They are commonly known as ecstasy.

Commonly known by the brand names Ritalin & Concerta, methylphenidate is similar in action to amphetamines and cocaine.

Causes of difficulty in sleeping

There are a great many possible reasons for sleeping poorly. Following sleep hygienic principles may solve problems of physical or emotional discomfort.[36] When pain, illness, drugs, or stress are the culprit, the cause must be treated. Sleep disorders, including the sleep apneas, narcolepsy, primary insomnia, periodic limb movement disorder (PLMD), restless leg syndrome (RLS) and the circadian rhythm sleep disorders, are treatable.

Elderly people may to some degree lose the ability to consolidate sleep. They need the same amount per day as they've always needed but may need to take some of their sleep as daytime naps.

Anthropology of sleep

Recent research suggests that sleep patterns vary significantly across cultures.[37][38] The most striking differences are between societies that have plentiful sources of artificial light and ones that do not. The primary difference appears to be that pre-light cultures have more broken up sleep patterns. For example, people might go to sleep far sooner after the sun sets, but then wake up several times throughout the night, punctuating their sleep with periods of wakefullness, perhaps lasting several hours. The boundaries between sleeping and waking are blurred in these societies. Some observers believe that sleep in these societies is most often split into two main periods, the first characterised primarily by deep sleep and the second by REM sleep. This segmented sleep has led to expressions such as "first sleep", "watch" and "second sleep" which appear in literature from pre-industrial societies all over the world.

Some societies display a fragmented sleep pattern in which people sleep at all times of the day and night for shorter periods. For example, many Mediterranean societies have a siesta, in which people sleep for a period in the afternoon. In many nomadic or hunter-gatherer societies people will sleep off and on throughout the day or night depending on what is happening. [39]

Plentiful artificial light has been available in the industrialised west since at least the mid-nineteenth century, and sleep patterns have changed significantly everywhere that lighting has been introduced. In general, people sleep in a more concentrated burst through the night, going to sleep much later, although this is not always true.

In some societies people generally sleep with at least one other person, often many, or with animals. In others people rarely sleep with anyone but a most intimate relation such as a spouse. In almost all societies sleeping partners are strongly regulated by social standards. For example, people might only sleep with their immediate family, extended family, spouses, with their children, with children of a certain age, children of specific gender, peers of a certain gender, friends, peers of equal social rank, or with no one at all. Sleep may be an actively social time depending on the sleep groupings, with no constraints on noise or activity. [40]

People sleep in a variety of locations. Some sleep directly on the ground, others on a skin or blanket, others sleep on platforms or beds. Some sleep with blankets, some with pillows, some with simple head rests, some with no head support. These choices are shaped by a variety of factors such as climate, protection from predators, housing type, technology, and the incidence of pests. [41]

Sleep in non-humans

Sleeping Japanese Macaques.

Horses and other herbivorous ungulates can sleep while standing, but must necessarily lie down for REM sleep (which causes muscular atony) for short periods - giraffes, for example, only need to lie down for REM sleep for a few minutes at a time. Bats sleep while hanging upside down. Some aquatic mammals and some birds can sleep with one half of the brain, while the other half is awake, so called unihemispheric slow-wave sleep.[42] Birds and mammals have cycles of non-REM and REM sleep as described above for humans, though birds’ cycles are much shorter and they do not lose muscle tone (go limp) to the same extent that most mammals do.

Many animals sleep, but neurological sleep states are difficult to define in lower order animals. In these animals, sleep is defined using behavioral characteristics such as minimal movement, postures typical for the species and reduced responsiveness to external stimulation. Sleep is quickly reversible, as opposed to hibernation or coma, and sleep deprivation is followed by longer and/or deeper sleep. Herbivores, who require a long waking period to gather and consume their diet, typically sleep less each day than similarly sized carnivores who might well consume several days supply of meat in a sitting.

Many species of mammals sleep for a large proportion of each 24-hour period when they are very young.[43] However, killer whales and some dolphins do not sleep during the first month of life.[44] Such differences may be explained by the ability of land mammal newborns to be easily protected by parents while sleeping, while marine animals must, even while very young, be more continuously vigilant for predators.

William C Dement in his book "The Promise of Sleep". states that the dolphin, originally a land mammal that has returned to the sea, maintains a number of terrestrial traits including bearing their offspring alive and, unlike fish, breathing air. When terrestrial mammals breathe, they do so through an involuntary process similar to the one that causes our hearts to beat continuously. In dolphins the breathing process is under voluntary control throughout the day --- a process that, seemingly, would preclude sleep. In order to accomplish what seems to be an impossible task, dolphins allow one half of their brain to go to sleep while the other half remains awake. This is accomplished in two-hour cycles where one half of the brain is awake while the other half sleeps until the dolphin's day's sleep need has been fulfilled.

See also

Common sleeping positions, practices, and rituals

Other

References

  1. ^ Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council. The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. pg 121. ISBN 978-0-309-08903-6. Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983). {{cite book}}: |pages= has extra text (help); Cite has empty unknown parameters: |accessyear=, |origmonth=, |accessmonth=, |chapterurl=, |month=, |origdate=, and |coauthors= (help)CS1 maint: others (link)
  2. ^ Bingham, Roger (2007). "Waking Up To Sleep" (Several conference videos). The Science Network. Retrieved 2008-01-25. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  3. ^ Swierzewski, Stanley J., M.D. (01 December 2000, reviewed 04 December 2007). "Sleep Stages. Overview, Waking, Non-REM, REM, Sleep Cycle, Factors, Age". Sleep Channel, Healthcommunities.com. Retrieved 2008-02-10. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  4. ^ Psychology World (1998). "Stages of Sleep" (PDF). Retrieved 2008-06-15. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  5. ^ Wyatt, James K. (1999). "Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day". Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. Fulltext. PMID 10516257. Retrieved 2007-11-25. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  6. ^ a b de Benedictis, Tina, Ph.D. (2007). "Understanding Sleep: Sleep Needs, Cycles, and Stages". Helpguide.org. Retrieved 2008-01-25. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  7. ^ Wyatt, James K. (1999). "Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day". Am J Physiol. 277 (4): R1152–R1163. PMID 10516257. Retrieved 2007-11-25. ... significant homeostatic and circadian modulation of sleep structure, with the highest sleep efficiency occurring in sleep episodes bracketing the melatonin maximum and core body temperature minimum {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  8. ^ a b ""Let Sleep Work for You" provided by the National Sleep Foundation".
  9. ^ Van Dongen HP, Maislin G, Mullington JM, Dinges DF. The cumulative cost of additional wakefulness: dose-response effects on neurobehavioral functions and sleep physiology from chronic sleep restriction and total sleep deprivation. Sleep. 2003 Mar 15;26(2):117–26.
  10. ^ Rhonda Rowland (2002-02-15). "Experts challenge study linking sleep, life span". Retrieved 2007-04-22.
  11. ^ Patel SR, Ayas NT, Malhotra MR et al (2004) A prospective study of sleep duration and mortality risk in women Sleep 1;27(3):440-4
  12. ^ Patel SR, Malhotra A, Gottlieb DJ et al (2006) Correlates of long sleep duration Sleep 29(7):881-889
  13. ^ Irwin MR, Ziegler M (2005) Sleep deprivation potentiates activation of cardiovascular and catecholamine responses in abstinent alcoholics Hypertension 45(2):252-7
  14. ^ ""Researchers say lack of sleep doubles risk of death… but so can too much sleep"".
  15. ^ Jane E. Ferrie, Martin J. Shipley, Francesco P. Cappuccio, Eric Brunner, Michelle A. Miller, Meena Kumari, and Michael G. Marmot. “A prospective study of change in sleep duration; associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort”. SLEEP Journal.
  16. ^ Siegel, Jerome M. (1999). "Sleep". Encarta Encyclopedia. Microsoft. Retrieved 2008-01-25.
  17. ^ Zisapel N.(2007) Sleep and sleep disturbances: biological basis and clinical implications. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences
  18. ^ Iglowstein, Ivo (2003). "Sleep Duration From Infancy to Adolescence: Reference Values and Generational Trends". Pediatrics. 111 (2): 302–307. doi:10.1542/peds.111.2.302. PMID 12563055. Thus, the shift in the evening bedtime across cohorts accounted for the substantial decrease in sleep duration in younger children between the 1970s and the 1990s. ... [A] more liberal parental attitude toward evening bedtime in the past decades is most likely responsible for the bedtime shift and for the decline of sleep duration... {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  19. ^ Gumustekin, K. (2004). "Effects of sleep deprivation, nicotine, and selenium on wound healing in rats" (Abstract). Int J Neurosci 2004;114(11): 1433-42. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |month= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  20. ^ Zager, A., Andersen, M. L., Ruiz, F. S., Antunes, I. B., & Tufik, S. (2007). Effects of acute and chronic sleep loss on immune modulation of rats [Electronic version]. Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology, 293, R504-R509.
  21. ^ Bonnet, M. H. & Arand, D. L. (2003). Insomnia, metabolic rate and sleep restoration [Electronic version]. Journal of Internal Medicine, 254, 23–31.
  22. ^ Jenni, O. G., Molinari, L., Caflisch, J. A., & Largo, R. H. (2007). Sleep duration from ages 1 to 10 years: Variability and stability in comparison with growth [Electronic version]. Pediatrics, 120, e769-e776.
  23. ^ Van Cauter, E., Leproult, R., & Plat, L. (2000). Age-related changes in slow-wave sleep and REM sleep and relationship with growth hormone and cortisol levels in healthy men [Electronic version]. Journal of the American Medical Association, 284, 861-868.
  24. ^ Daan S, Barnes BM, Strijkstra AM (1991). "Warming up for sleep? Ground squirrels sleep during arousals from hibernation". Neurosci. Lett. 128 (2): 581. doi:10.1016/0304-3940(91)90276-Y. PMID 1945046.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ Guidelines for the Care and Use of Mammals in Neuroscience and Behavioral Research. Institute for Laboratory Animal Research (ILAR), National Research Council. The National Academies Press. 2003. pp. pg 121. ISBN 978-0-309-08903-6. Sleep deprivation of over 7 days with the disk-over-water system results in the development of ulcerative skin lesions, hyperphagia, loss of body mass, hypothermia, and eventually septicemia and death in rats (Everson, 1995; Rechtschaffen et al., 1983). {{cite book}}: |pages= has extra text (help); Cite has empty unknown parameters: |accessyear=, |origmonth=, |accessmonth=, |chapterurl=, |month=, |origdate=, and |coauthors= (help)CS1 maint: others (link)
  26. ^ Amanda Schaffer (Mai 27, 2007). "Why do we Sleep?". Slate.com. Retrieved 2008-08-23. {{cite web}}: Check date values in: |date= (help)
  27. ^ Turner, T. H., Drummond, S. P. A., Salamat, J. S., & Brown, G. G. (2007). Effects of 42 hr sleep deprivation on component processes of verbal working memory [Electronic version]. Neuropsychology, 21, 787-795.
  28. ^ Born, J., Rasch, J., & Gais, S. (2006). Sleep to remember [Electronic version]. Neuroscientist, 12, 410.
  29. ^ Datta, S. (2000). Avoidance task training potentiates phasic pontine-wave density in the rat: A mechanism for sleep-dependent plasticity [Electronic version]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 8607-8613.
  30. ^ Kudrimoti, H. S., Barnes, C. A., & McNaughton, B. L. (1999). Reactivation of hippocampal cell assemblies: Effects of behavioral state, experience, and EEG dynamics [Electronic version]. The Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 4090-4101.
  31. ^ "Nature, 10/05
  32. ^ Nanayakkara, Anushka. "Dream your dreams away". The Observer. Retrieved 2008-07-09.
  33. ^ Hobson, J. A., & McCarley, R. (1977). The brain as a dream state of sex: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. American Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 1335–1348.
  34. ^ Abarca C, Albrecht U, Spanagel R. Cocaine sensitization and reward are under the influence of circadian genes and rhythm. Department of Psychopharmacology, Central Institute of Mental Health, University of Heidelberg, J5, 68159 Mannheim, Germany.
  35. ^ Primary hypersomnia: Diagnostic Features
  36. ^ Little, Nan (2007-01-01). "What Causes Sleep Difficulty?". Insight Journal. Retrieved 2008-01-25.
  37. ^ Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby. "6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep". A comparative developmental ecology. Emory University. {{cite book}}: |format= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  38. ^ Slumber's Unexplored Landscape, Science News Online (9/25/99)
  39. ^ Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby. "6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep". A comparative developmental ecology. Emory University. {{cite book}}: |format= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  40. ^ Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby. "6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep". A comparative developmental ecology. Emory University. {{cite book}}: |format= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  41. ^ Carol M. Worthman and Melissa K. Melby. "6. Toward a comparative developmental ecology of human sleep". A comparative developmental ecology. Emory University. {{cite book}}: |format= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  42. ^ Mukhametova LM (1977-10-14). "Interhemispheric asymmetry of the electroencephalographic sleep patterns in dolphins". Brain Research. 134 (3): 581–584. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90835-6. PMID 902119. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  43. ^ Faraco, Juliette (2000-08-01). "Re: Are there animals who don't sleep or that sleep very little?". MadSci Network: Zoology. Retrieved 2008-01-25. {{cite web}}: Cite has empty unknown parameter: |coauthors= (help)
  44. ^ Insomnia Mania: Newborn Mammals Don't Sleep for a Month | LiveScience

Further reading

  • Bar-Yam, Yaneer (2003). "Chapter 3". Dynamics of Complex Systems. {{cite book}}: |format= requires |url= (help); External link in |chapterurl= (help); Unknown parameter |chapterurl= ignored (|chapter-url= suggested) (help)
  • Foldvary-Schaefer N, Grigg-Damberger M (2006). "Sleep and epilepsy: what we know, don't know, and need to know". J Clin Neurophysiol. 23 (1): 4–20. doi:10.1097/01.wnp.0000206877.90232.cb. PMID 16514348. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  • Gilmartin G, Thomas R (2004). "Mechanisms of arousal from sleep and their consequences". Curr Opin Pulm Med. 10 (6): 468–74. doi:10.1097/01.mcp.0000143690.94442.b3. PMID 15510052. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help) [Review]
  • Gottlieb D, Punjabi N, Newman A, Resnick H, Redline S, Baldwin C, Nieto F (2005). "Association of sleep time with diabetes mellitus and impaired glucose tolerance". Arch Intern Med. 165 (8): 863–7. doi:10.1001/archinte.165.8.863. PMID 15851636. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Legramante J, Galante A (2005). "Sleep and hypertension: a challenge for the autonomic regulation of the cardiovascular system". Circulation. 112 (6): 786–8. doi:10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.105.555714. PMID 16087808. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help) [Editorial]
  • Feinberg I. Changes in sleep cycle patterns with age J Psychiatr Res. 1974;10:283–306. [review]
  • Dement, William C., M.D., Ph.D. The Promise of Sleep. Delacorte Press, Random House Inc., New York, 1999.
  • Tamar Shochat and Sonia Ancoli - Specific Clinical Patterns in Aging - Sleep and Sleep Disorders [website]
  • Zepelin H. Normal age related changes in sleep. In: Chase M, Weitzman ED, eds. Sleep Disorders: Basic and Clinical Research. New York: SP Medical; 1983:431–434.
  • Morrissey M, Duntley S, Anch A, Nonneman R (2004). "Active sleep and its role in the prevention of apoptosis in the developing brain". Med Hypotheses. 62 (6): 876–9. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2004.01.014. PMID 15142640.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Marks G, Shaffery J, Oksenberg A, Speciale S, Roffwarg H (1995). "A functional role for REM sleep in brain maturation". Behav Brain Res. 69 (1–2): 1–11. doi:10.1016/0166-4328(95)00018-O. PMID 7546299. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Mirmiran M, Scholtens J, van de Poll N, Uylings H, van der Gugten J, Boer G (1983). "Effects of experimental suppression of active (REM) sleep during early development upon adult brain and behavior in the rat". Brain Res. 283 (2–3): 277–86. PMID 6850353. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Zhang, J. (2004). "[Memory process and the function of sleep]" (PDF). Journal of Theoretics. 6 (6). {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)

External links