Jump to content

Rhoticity in English: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
rv- The tag is in the appropriate spot. It seems that this specific line may need clarification.
Undid revision 787651053 by LakeKayak (talk): no, the claims are more or less identical, and the tag should be in the text rather than in the summary at the top
Line 6: Line 6:
In non-rhotic varieties, speakers no longer pronounce {{IPA|/r/}} in [[postvocalic]] environments – that is, when it is immediately after a vowel and not followed by another vowel.<ref>Paul Skandera, Peter Burleigh, ''A Manual of English Phonetics and Phonology'', Gunter Narr Verlag, 2011, p. 60.</ref>{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} For example, a rhotic English speaker pronounces the words ''hard'' and ''butter'' as {{IPAc-en|ˈ|h|ɑɹ|d}} and {{IPAc-en|ˈ|b|ʌ|t|əɹ}}, whereas a non-rhotic speaker "drops" or "deletes" the {{IPA|/r/}} sound, pronouncing them as {{IPAc-en|ˈ|h|ɑː|d}} and {{IPAc-en|ˈ|b|ʌ|t|ə}}. A non-rhotic speaker usually still pronounces the {{IPA|/r/}} in the continuously spoken phrase "butte''r a''nd jam" (the [[linking R]]), since in this case the {{IPA|/r/}} is followed by a vowel.
In non-rhotic varieties, speakers no longer pronounce {{IPA|/r/}} in [[postvocalic]] environments – that is, when it is immediately after a vowel and not followed by another vowel.<ref>Paul Skandera, Peter Burleigh, ''A Manual of English Phonetics and Phonology'', Gunter Narr Verlag, 2011, p. 60.</ref>{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} For example, a rhotic English speaker pronounces the words ''hard'' and ''butter'' as {{IPAc-en|ˈ|h|ɑɹ|d}} and {{IPAc-en|ˈ|b|ʌ|t|əɹ}}, whereas a non-rhotic speaker "drops" or "deletes" the {{IPA|/r/}} sound, pronouncing them as {{IPAc-en|ˈ|h|ɑː|d}} and {{IPAc-en|ˈ|b|ʌ|t|ə}}. A non-rhotic speaker usually still pronounces the {{IPA|/r/}} in the continuously spoken phrase "butte''r a''nd jam" (the [[linking R]]), since in this case the {{IPA|/r/}} is followed by a vowel.


Loss of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} began sporadically in informal English of the 1400s, and by the 1600s postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} was weakened{{clarification needed|date=June 2017}} but still universally present. In the mid-1700s it was still pronounced in most environments, but may occasionally have been deleted entirely, especially after low vowels. By the late 1700s, non-rhotic pronunciation had become common in London and surrounding areas, and was being increasingly used even in more formal and educated speech. By the early 1800s, the [[Received Pronunciation|southern British standard]] was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though some variation persisted as late as the 1870s. This loss of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} in British English influenced port cities of the United States with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern American port cities to become non-rhotic. Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American [[Prestige (sociolinguistics)|prestige speech]], though the [[American Civil War]] of the 1860s began to shift America's centers of wealth and political power to rhotic areas with fewer cultural connections to the old colonial and British elites. The advent of [[radio]] and [[television]] in the early 1900s established a [[General American|national media standard]] of American pronunciation which, particularly right after World War II, solidified as a rhotic variety that fully preserved historical {{IPA|/r/}}.
Loss of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} began sporadically in informal English of the 1400s, and by the 1600s postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} was weakened but still universally present. In the mid-1700s it was still pronounced in most environments, but may occasionally have been deleted entirely, especially after low vowels. By the late 1700s, non-rhotic pronunciation had become common in London and surrounding areas, and was being increasingly used even in more formal and educated speech. By the early 1800s, the [[Received Pronunciation|southern British standard]] was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though some variation persisted as late as the 1870s. This loss of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}} in British English influenced port cities of the United States with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern American port cities to become non-rhotic. Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American [[Prestige (sociolinguistics)|prestige speech]], though the [[American Civil War]] of the 1860s began to shift America's centers of wealth and political power to rhotic areas with fewer cultural connections to the old colonial and British elites. The advent of [[radio]] and [[television]] in the early 1900s established a [[General American|national media standard]] of American pronunciation which, particularly right after World War II, solidified as a rhotic variety that fully preserved historical {{IPA|/r/}}.


Other terms for "rhotic" varieties include '''{{IPA|/r/}}–pronouncing''' or '''''r''–ful''',{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}}<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47">Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006): 47.</ref> and for "non-rhotic" include '''{{IPA|/r/}}-deleting''',{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} '''r-dropping''',{{sfnp|Wells|1982|p=216}} '''''r''-vocalized''', or '''''r''–less'''.<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47"/>
Other terms for "rhotic" varieties include '''{{IPA|/r/}}–pronouncing''' or '''''r''–ful''',{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}}<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47">Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006): 47.</ref> and for "non-rhotic" include '''{{IPA|/r/}}-deleting''',{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} '''r-dropping''',{{sfnp|Wells|1982|p=216}} '''''r''-vocalized''', or '''''r''–less'''.<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47"/>
Line 16: Line 16:
The earliest traces of a loss of {{IPA|/r/}} in English appear in the early 1400s and occur before [[coronal consonant]]s, especially {{IPA|/s/}}, giving modern "ass (buttocks)" ([[Old English]] ''ears'', [[Middle English]] ''ers'' or ''ars''), and "[[bass (fish)]]" (OE ''bærs'', ME ''bars'').{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} A second phase of {{IPA|/r/}}-loss began during the 1400s, and was characterized by sporadic and lexically variable deletion, such as ''monyng'' "morning" and ''cadenall'' "cardinal".{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} These {{IPA|/r/}}-less spellings appear throughout the 16th and 17th centuries, but are uncommon and are restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} No English authorities describe loss of {{IPA|/r/}} in the standard language prior to the mid-1700s, and many do not fully accept it until the 1790s.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}}
The earliest traces of a loss of {{IPA|/r/}} in English appear in the early 1400s and occur before [[coronal consonant]]s, especially {{IPA|/s/}}, giving modern "ass (buttocks)" ([[Old English]] ''ears'', [[Middle English]] ''ers'' or ''ars''), and "[[bass (fish)]]" (OE ''bærs'', ME ''bars'').{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} A second phase of {{IPA|/r/}}-loss began during the 1400s, and was characterized by sporadic and lexically variable deletion, such as ''monyng'' "morning" and ''cadenall'' "cardinal".{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} These {{IPA|/r/}}-less spellings appear throughout the 16th and 17th centuries, but are uncommon and are restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} No English authorities describe loss of {{IPA|/r/}} in the standard language prior to the mid-1700s, and many do not fully accept it until the 1790s.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}}


During the mid-1600s, a number of sources describe {{IPA|/r/}} as being weakened but still present.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} The English playwright [[Ben Jonson]]'s ''English Grammar'', published posthumously in 1640, records that {{IPA|/r/}} was "sounded firme in the beginning of words, and more liquid in the middle, and ends."{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} Little more is said regarding {{IPA|/r/}} until 1740, when one Mather Flint, writing in a primer for French learners of English, said: ''"...dans plusieurs mots, l’r devant une consonne est fort adouci, presque muet & rend un peu longue la voyale qui le precede."'' ("...in many words ''r'' before a consonant is greatly softened, almost mute, and slightly lengthens the preceding vowel."){{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} By the 1770s, postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}}-less pronunciation was becoming common around London even in more formal, educated speech. The English actor and linguist [[John Walker (lexicographer)|John Walker]] uses the spelling ''ar'' to indicate the long vowel of ''aunt'' in his 1775 rhyming dictionary.<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47"/> In his influential ''Critical Pronouncing Dictionary and Expositor of the English Language'' (1791), Walker reported, with a strong tone of disapproval, that "...the ''r'' in ''lard'', ''bard'', [...] is pronounced so much in the throat as to be little more than the middle or Italian ''a'', lengthened into ''baa'', ''baad''...."{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} Americans returning to England after the end of the [[American Revolutionary War]] in 1783 reported surprise at the significant changes in fashionable pronunciation.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=73}} By the early 1800s, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though it continued to be variable as late as the 1870s.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}}
During the mid-1600s, a number of sources describe {{IPA|/r/}} as being weakened{{clarification needed|date=June 2017}} but still present.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=114}} The English playwright [[Ben Jonson]]'s ''English Grammar'', published posthumously in 1640, records that {{IPA|/r/}} was "sounded firme in the beginning of words, and more liquid in the middle, and ends."{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} Little more is said regarding {{IPA|/r/}} until 1740, when one Mather Flint, writing in a primer for French learners of English, said: ''"...dans plusieurs mots, l’r devant une consonne est fort adouci, presque muet & rend un peu longue la voyale qui le precede."'' ("...in many words ''r'' before a consonant is greatly softened, almost mute, and slightly lengthens the preceding vowel."){{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} By the 1770s, postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}}-less pronunciation was becoming common around London even in more formal, educated speech. The English actor and linguist [[John Walker (lexicographer)|John Walker]] uses the spelling ''ar'' to indicate the long vowel of ''aunt'' in his 1775 rhyming dictionary.<ref name="Labov, Ash, and Boberg 2006: 47"/> In his influential ''Critical Pronouncing Dictionary and Expositor of the English Language'' (1791), Walker reported, with a strong tone of disapproval, that "...the ''r'' in ''lard'', ''bard'', [...] is pronounced so much in the throat as to be little more than the middle or Italian ''a'', lengthened into ''baa'', ''baad''...."{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}} Americans returning to England after the end of the [[American Revolutionary War]] in 1783 reported surprise at the significant changes in fashionable pronunciation.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=73}} By the early 1800s, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though it continued to be variable as late as the 1870s.{{sfnp|Lass|1999|p=115}}


The adoption of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}}-less pronunciation as the British prestige standard in the late 18th and early 19th centuries influenced American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern port cities such as [[New York City]], [[Boston]], [[Alexandria, Virginia|Alexandria]], [[Charleston, South Carolina|Charleston]], and [[Savannah, Georgia|Savannah]] to become non-rhotic.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=76}} Like regional dialects in England, the accents of other areas in America remained rhotic in a display of linguistic "lag" that preserved the original pronunciation of {{IPA|/r/}}.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=76}} Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the [[American Civil War]] shifted America's centers of wealth and political power to areas with fewer cultural connections to the British elite.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=77}} Labov et al report that non-rhotic remained the prestige speech until World War II.<ref>{{Harvp|Labov|Ash|Boberg|2006|loc=chpt 7}}</ref>
The adoption of postvocalic {{IPA|/r/}}-less pronunciation as the British prestige standard in the late 18th and early 19th centuries influenced American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern port cities such as [[New York City]], [[Boston]], [[Alexandria, Virginia|Alexandria]], [[Charleston, South Carolina|Charleston]], and [[Savannah, Georgia|Savannah]] to become non-rhotic.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=76}} Like regional dialects in England, the accents of other areas in America remained rhotic in a display of linguistic "lag" that preserved the original pronunciation of {{IPA|/r/}}.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=76}} Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the [[American Civil War]] shifted America's centers of wealth and political power to areas with fewer cultural connections to the British elite.{{sfnp|Fisher|2001|p=77}} Labov et al report that non-rhotic remained the prestige speech until World War II.<ref>{{Harvp|Labov|Ash|Boberg|2006|loc=chpt 7}}</ref>

Revision as of 19:35, 26 June 2017

Rhoticity in English refers to the situations in which English speakers pronounce the historical rhotic consonant /r/, and a presence or absence of rhoticity is one of the most prominent distinctions by which varieties of English can be classified. The English dialects of Scotland, Ireland, and most of the United States and Canada preserve historical /r/, and are thus termed the rhotic varieties. The non-rhotic varieties, in which historical /r/ is not pronounced, except before vowels, include all the dialects of England—except the South West, the southern West Midlands, and parts of West Lancashire—as well as the English dialects of Australia, New Zealand, South Africa, and some parts of the southern and eastern coastal United States.

In non-rhotic varieties, speakers no longer pronounce /r/ in postvocalic environments – that is, when it is immediately after a vowel and not followed by another vowel.[1][2] For example, a rhotic English speaker pronounces the words hard and butter as /ˈhɑːrd/ and /ˈbʌtər/, whereas a non-rhotic speaker "drops" or "deletes" the /r/ sound, pronouncing them as /ˈhɑːd/ and /ˈbʌtə/. A non-rhotic speaker usually still pronounces the /r/ in the continuously spoken phrase "butter and jam" (the linking R), since in this case the /r/ is followed by a vowel.

Loss of postvocalic /r/ began sporadically in informal English of the 1400s, and by the 1600s postvocalic /r/ was weakened but still universally present. In the mid-1700s it was still pronounced in most environments, but may occasionally have been deleted entirely, especially after low vowels. By the late 1700s, non-rhotic pronunciation had become common in London and surrounding areas, and was being increasingly used even in more formal and educated speech. By the early 1800s, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though some variation persisted as late as the 1870s. This loss of postvocalic /r/ in British English influenced port cities of the United States with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern American port cities to become non-rhotic. Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech, though the American Civil War of the 1860s began to shift America's centers of wealth and political power to rhotic areas with fewer cultural connections to the old colonial and British elites. The advent of radio and television in the early 1900s established a national media standard of American pronunciation which, particularly right after World War II, solidified as a rhotic variety that fully preserved historical /r/.

Other terms for "rhotic" varieties include /r/–pronouncing or r–ful,[2][3] and for "non-rhotic" include /r/-deleting,[2] r-dropping,[4] r-vocalized, or r–less.[3]

History

Red areas indicate where rural accents were rhotic in the 1950s. Based on H. Orton et al., Survey of English Dialects (1962–71). Some areas with partial rhoticity (for example parts of the East Riding of Yorkshire) are not shaded on this map.
Red areas are where English dialects of the late 1900s were rhotic. Based on P. Trudgill, The Dialects of England.

The earliest traces of a loss of /r/ in English appear in the early 1400s and occur before coronal consonants, especially /s/, giving modern "ass (buttocks)" (Old English ears, Middle English ers or ars), and "bass (fish)" (OE bærs, ME bars).[2] A second phase of /r/-loss began during the 1400s, and was characterized by sporadic and lexically variable deletion, such as monyng "morning" and cadenall "cardinal".[2] These /r/-less spellings appear throughout the 16th and 17th centuries, but are uncommon and are restricted to private documents, especially ones written by women.[2] No English authorities describe loss of /r/ in the standard language prior to the mid-1700s, and many do not fully accept it until the 1790s.[2]

During the mid-1600s, a number of sources describe /r/ as being weakened[clarification needed] but still present.[2] The English playwright Ben Jonson's English Grammar, published posthumously in 1640, records that /r/ was "sounded firme in the beginning of words, and more liquid in the middle, and ends."[5] Little more is said regarding /r/ until 1740, when one Mather Flint, writing in a primer for French learners of English, said: "...dans plusieurs mots, l’r devant une consonne est fort adouci, presque muet & rend un peu longue la voyale qui le precede." ("...in many words r before a consonant is greatly softened, almost mute, and slightly lengthens the preceding vowel.")[5] By the 1770s, postvocalic /r/-less pronunciation was becoming common around London even in more formal, educated speech. The English actor and linguist John Walker uses the spelling ar to indicate the long vowel of aunt in his 1775 rhyming dictionary.[3] In his influential Critical Pronouncing Dictionary and Expositor of the English Language (1791), Walker reported, with a strong tone of disapproval, that "...the r in lard, bard, [...] is pronounced so much in the throat as to be little more than the middle or Italian a, lengthened into baa, baad...."[5] Americans returning to England after the end of the American Revolutionary War in 1783 reported surprise at the significant changes in fashionable pronunciation.[6] By the early 1800s, the southern British standard was fully transformed into a non-rhotic variety, though it continued to be variable as late as the 1870s.[5]

The adoption of postvocalic /r/-less pronunciation as the British prestige standard in the late 18th and early 19th centuries influenced American port cities with close connections to Britain, causing upper-class pronunciation in many eastern and southern port cities such as New York City, Boston, Alexandria, Charleston, and Savannah to become non-rhotic.[7] Like regional dialects in England, the accents of other areas in America remained rhotic in a display of linguistic "lag" that preserved the original pronunciation of /r/.[7] Non-rhotic pronunciation continued to influence American prestige speech until the 1860s, when the American Civil War shifted America's centers of wealth and political power to areas with fewer cultural connections to the British elite.[8] Labov et al report that non-rhotic remained the prestige speech until World War II.[9]

Modern pronunciation

In most non-rhotic accents, if a word ending in written "r" is followed closely by a word beginning with a vowel, the /r/ is pronounced—as in water ice. This phenomenon is referred to as "linking R". Many non-rhotic speakers also insert an epenthetic /r/ between vowels when the first vowel is one that can occur before syllable-final r (drawring for drawing). This so-called "intrusive R" has been stigmatized, but nowadays many speakers of Received Pronunciation (RP) frequently "intrude" an epenthetic /r/ at word boundaries, especially where one or both vowels is schwa; for example the idea of it becomes the idea-r-of it, Australia and New Zealand becomes Australia-r-and New Zealand, the formerly well-known India-r-Office and "Laura Norder" (Law and Order). The typical alternative used by RP speakers (and some rhotic speakers as well) is to insert a glottal stop where an intrusive R would otherwise be placed.[10][11]

For non-rhotic speakers, what was historically a vowel plus /r/ is now usually realized as a long vowel. This is called compensatory lengthening, lengthening that occurs after the elision of a sound. So in RP and many other non-rhotic accents card, fern, born are pronounced [kʰɑːd], [fɜːn], [bɔːn] or similar (actual pronunciations vary from accent to accent). This length may be retained in phrases, so while car pronounced in isolation is [kʰɑː], car owner is [ˈkʰɑːɹəʊnə]. But a final schwa usually remains short, so water in isolation is [wɔːtʰə]. In RP and similar accents the vowels /iː/ and /uː/ (or /ʊ/), when followed by r, become diphthongs ending in schwa, so near is [nɪə] and poor is [pʰʊə], though these have other realizations as well, including monophthongal ones; once again, the pronunciations vary from accent to accent. The same happens to diphthongs followed by R, though these may be considered to end in /ər/ in rhotic speech, and it is the /ər/ that reduces to schwa as usual in non-rhotic speech: tire said in isolation is [tʰaɪə] and sour is [saʊə].[12] For some speakers, some long vowels alternate with a diphthong ending in schwa, so wear may be [wɛə] but wearing [wɛːɹɪŋ].

Even General American speakers commonly drop the /r/ in non-final unstressed syllables when another syllable in the same word also contains /r/; this may be referred to as R-dissimilation. Examples include the dropping of the first /r/ in the words surprise, governor and caterpillar. In more careful speech, however, the /r/ sounds are all retained.[13]

Distribution

Final post-vocalic /r/ in farmer in English rural dialects of the 1950s[14]
GREEN[ə] (non-rhotic)
YELLOW[əʴ] (alveolar)
ORANGE[əʵ] (retroflex)
PINK[əʵː] (retroflex & long)
BLUE[əʶ] (uvular)
VIOLET[ɔʶ] (back & rounded)

Rhotic accents include Scottish English, Irish or Hiberno-English, most varieties of North American English, Barbadian English, Indian English,[15] and Pakistani English.[16]

Non-rhotic accents include most English English, Welsh English, New Zealand English, Australian English, and South African English.

Semi-rhotic accents have also been studied, such as Jamaican English, in which r is pronounced (as in even non-rhotic accents) before vowels, but also in stressed monosyllables or stressed syllables at the ends of words (e.g. in "car" or "dare"); however, it is not pronounced at the end of unstressed syllables (e.g. in "water") or before consonants (e.g. "market").[17]

Variably rhotic accents, in which speakers often sporadically waver between rhoticity and non-rhoticity without any particular rules of context, are also widely documented. Variably rhotic accents comprise much of Caribbean English, for example, as spoken in Tobago, Guyana, Antigua and Barbuda, and the Bahamas.[18] They also include current-day New York City English,[19] New York Latino English, and some Boston English.

Non-rhotic accents in the Americas include the rest of Caribbean and Belize.

England

Though most English varieties in England are non-rhotic today, stemming from a trend towards this in southeastern England accelerating in the very late 1700s onwards, rhotic accents are still found in the West Country (south and west of a line from near Shrewsbury to around Portsmouth), the Corby area, some of Lancashire (north and west of the centre of Manchester), some parts of Yorkshire and Lincolnshire, and in the areas that border Scotland. The prestige form, however, exerts a steady pressure towards non-rhoticity. Thus the urban speech of Bristol or Southampton is more accurately described as variably rhotic, the degree of rhoticity being reduced as one moves up the class and formality scales.[20]

The red dots show major U.S. cities where Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006:48) found 50% or higher non-rhotic speech among at least one white resident. African American Vernacular English's non-rhotic speech may be found among working- and middle-class African Americans throughout the country.

United States

American English is predominantly rhotic today, but at the end of the 1800s non-rhotic accents were common throughout much of the Eastern U.S. and through much of the South along the Gulf Coast. In fact, non-rhotic accents were established in all major U.S. cities along the Atlantic coast except for the Delaware Valley area around Philadelphia and Baltimore. This trend reversed during the early to mid-1900s. Non-rhotic accents have increasingly been perceived by Americans as sounding foreign or less educated with rhotic accents increasingly seen as sounding more "General American".[21]

Today, non-rhoticity in the American South is found primarily among older speakers, and only in some areas such as central and southern Alabama; Savannah, Georgia; and Norfolk, Virginia,[22] as well as in the Yat accent of New Orleans. The local dialects of eastern New England, especially Boston, Massachusetts, extending into the states of Maine and New Hampshire, are largely non-rhotic, as well as the traditional Rhode Island dialect; however, this feature has recently been receding. The New York City dialect is traditionally non-rhotic, though William Labov more precisely classifies its current form as variably rhotic,[23] with many of its sub-varieties now fully rhotic, such as in northeastern New Jersey.

African American Vernacular English (AAVE for short) is largely non-rhotic, and in some non-rhotic Southern and AAVE accents, there is no linking r, that is, /r/ at the end of a word is deleted even when the following word starts with a vowel, so that "Mister Adams" is pronounced [mɪstə(ʔ)ˈædəmz].[24] In a few such accents, intervocalic /r/ is deleted before an unstressed syllable even within a word when the following syllable begins with a vowel. In such accents, pronunciations like [kæəˈlaːnə] for Carolina, or [bɛːˈʌp] for "bear up" are heard.[25] This pronunciation also occurs in AAVE.[26] This also occurred for many older non-rhotic Southern speakers.[27]

Typically, even non-rhotic modern American English varieties do pronounce the /r/ in /ɜːr/ (as in "bird," "work," or "perky"), realizing it, as in most of the U.S., as [ɝ] or [ɚ] listen.

Canada

Canadian English is entirely rhotic except for small isolated areas in southwestern New Brunswick, parts of Newfoundland, and the Lunenburg English variety spoken in Lunenburg and Shelburne Counties, Nova Scotia, which may be non-rhotic or variably rhotic.[28]

New Zealand

Although New Zealand English is predominantly non-rhotic, Southland and parts of Otago in the far south of New Zealand's South Island are rhotic from apparent Scottish influence. Some Māori speakers are semi-rhotic although it is not clearly identified to any particular region or attributed to any defined language shift. The Māori accent varies from the European-origin New Zealand accent.

Asia

The English spoken in Asia is predominantly rhotic. Many varieties of Indian English are rhotic owing to the underlying phonotactics of the native Indo-Aryan and Dravidian languages[15] whilst some tend to be non-rhotic. In the case of the Philippines, this may be explained because the English that is spoken there is heavily influenced by the American dialect. In addition, many East Asians (in China, Japan, Korea, and Taiwan) who have a good command of English generally have rhotic accents because of the influence of American English. This excludes Hong Kong, whose RP English dialect is a result of its almost 150-year-history as a British Crown colony (later British dependent territory). The lack of consonant /r/ in Cantonese also contribute to the phenomenon (although rhoticity started to exist due to the handover in 1997 and influence by US and East Asian entertainment industry). However, many older (and younger) speakers among South and East Asians speak non-rhotic.

Other Asian regions with non-rhotic English are Malaysia, Singapore, and Brunei.[citation needed] A typical Malaysian's English would be almost totally non-rhotic due to the nonexistence of rhotic endings in both languages of influence, whereas a more educated Malaysian's English may be non-rhotic due to Standard Malaysian English being based on RP (Received Pronunciation).

A typical teenager's Southeast Asian English would be rhotic, mainly because of prominent influence by American English. Spoken English in Myanmar is non-rhotic, but there are a number of English speakers with a rhotic or partially rhotic pronunciation. Sri Lankan English may be rhotic.

Africa

The English spoken in Africa (excluding South Africa) is based on British RP and is officially non-rhotic. Pronunciation and variation in African English accents are largely affected by native African language influences, level of education and exposure to western influences. The English accents spoken in the coastal areas of West Africa is primarily non-rhotic as are the underlying varieties of Niger-Congo languages spoken in that part of West Africa. Rhoticity may be present in English spoken in areas where rhotic Afro-Asiatic or Nilo Saharan languages are spoken across northern West Africa and in the Nilotic regions of East Africa. More modern trends show an increasing African American influence on African English pronunciation particularly among younger urban affluent populations, where the American rhotic 'r' may be over-stressed in informal communication to create a pseudo-Americanised accent. By and large official spoken English used in post colonial African countries is non-rhotic. Standard Liberian English is also non-rhotic because liquids are lost at the end of words or before consonants.[29]

Mergers characteristic of non-rhotic accents

Some phonemic mergers are characteristic of non-rhotic accents. These usually include one item that historically contained an R (lost in the non-rhotic accent), and one that never did so. The section below lists mergers in order of approximately decreasing prevalence.

Panda–pander merger

In the terminology of John C. Wells, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets commA and lettER. It is found in all or nearly all non-rhotic accents,[30] and is even present in some accents that are in other respects rhotic, such as those of some speakers in Jamaica and the Bahamas.[30]

Father–farther merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets PALM and START. It is found in the speech of the great majority of non-rhotic speakers, including those of England, Wales, the United States, the Caribbean, Australia, New Zealand and South Africa. It may be absent in some non-rhotic speakers in the Bahamas.[30]

Pawn–porn merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and NORTH. It is found in most of the same accents as the father–farther merger described above, but is absent from the Bahamas and Guyana.[30]

Caught–court merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and FORCE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the pawnporn merger that have also undergone the horse–hoarse merger. These include the accents of Southern England, Wales, non-rhotic New York City speakers, Trinidad and the Southern hemisphere. In such accents a three-way merger awe-or-ore/oar results. However, Labov et al. suggest that, in New York City English, this merger is present in perception not production. As in, although even locals perceive themselves using the same vowel in both cases, they tend to produce the force higher and more retracted than the vowel of thought.[31]

Calve–carve merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets BATH and START. It is found in some non-rhotic accents with broad A in words like "bath". It is general in southern England (excluding rhotic speakers), Trinidad, the Bahamas, and the Southern hemisphere. It is a possibility for Welsh, Eastern New England, Jamaican, and Guyanese speakers.

Paw–poor merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets THOUGHT and CURE. It is found in those non-rhotic accents containing the caughtcourt merger that have also undergone the pour–poor merger. Wells lists it unequivocally only for the accent of Trinidad, but it is an option for non-rhotic speakers in England, Australia and New Zealand. Such speakers have a potential four–way merger taw-tor-tore-tour.[32]

Batted–battered merger

This merger is present in non-rhotic accents which have undergone the weak-vowel merger. Such accents include Australian, New Zealand, most South African speech, and some non-rhotic English speech (e.g. Norfolk, Sheffield).

A large number of homophonous pairs involve the syllabic -es and agentive -ers suffixes, such as merges-mergers and bleaches-bleachers. Because there are so many, they are excluded from the list of homophonous pairs below.

Dough–door merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and FORCE. It may be found in some southern U.S. non-rhotic speech, some speakers of African American Vernacular English, some speakers in Guyana and some Welsh speech.[30]

Show–sure merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets GOAT and CURE. It may be present in those speakers who have both the dough–door merger described above, and also the pour–poor merger. These include some southern U.S. non-rhotic speakers, some speakers of African American Vernacular English and some speakers in Guyana.[30]
It can be seen in the term "Fo Sho", an imitation of "for sure".

Often–orphan merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CLOTH and NORTH. It may be present in old-fashioned Eastern New England accents,[33] New York City speakers[34] and also in some speakers in Jamaica and Guyana. The merger was also until recently present in the dialects of southern England, including Received Pronunciation—specifically, the phonemic merger of the words often and orphan was a running gag in the Gilbert and Sullivan musical, The Pirates of Penzance.

God–guard merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and START. It may be present in non-rhotic accents that have undergone the father–bother merger. These may include some New York accents,[36] some southern U.S. accents,[37] and African American Vernacular English.[38]

Shot–short merger

In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets LOT and NORTH. It may be present in some Eastern New England accents.[39][40]

Bud–bird merger

A merger of /ɜː(r)/ and /ʌ/ occurring for some speakers of Jamaican English making bud and bird homophones as /bʌd/.[41] The conversion of /ɜː/ to [ʌ] or [ə] is also found in places scattered around England and Scotland. Some speakers, mostly rural, in the area from London to Norfolk exhibit this conversion, mainly before voiceless fricatives. This gives pronunciation like first [fʌst] and worse [wʌs]. The word cuss appears to derive from the application of this sound change to the word curse. Similarly, lurve is coined from love.
In Wells' terminology, this consists of the merger of the lexical sets CHOICE and NURSE preconsonantally (so that "stir" and "boy" never rhymed). It was present in older New York and New Orleans regional accents, but became stigmatized and is sharply recessive in those born since the Second World War.[42] This merger is known for the word soitanly, used often by the Three Stooges comedian Curly Howard as a variant of certainly in comedy shorts of the 1930s and 1940s.

Other mergers

In some accents, syllabification may interact with rhoticity, resulting in homophones where non-rhotic accents have centering diphthongs. Possibilities include Korea–career,[43] Shi'a–sheer, and Maia–mire,[44] while skua may be identical with the second syllable of obscure.[45]

Effect of non-rhotic dialects on orthography

Template:ChineseText Template:Contains Indic text Template:KoreanText Template:Contains Urdu text Certain words have spellings derived from non-rhotic dialects or renderings of foreign words through non-rhotic pronunciation. Examples include:

See also

References

Footnotes

  1. ^ Paul Skandera, Peter Burleigh, A Manual of English Phonetics and Phonology, Gunter Narr Verlag, 2011, p. 60.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h Lass (1999), p. 114.
  3. ^ a b c Labov, Ash, and Boberg (2006): 47.
  4. ^ Wells (1982), p. 216. sfnp error: multiple targets (2×): CITEREFWells1982 (help)
  5. ^ a b c d Lass (1999), p. 115.
  6. ^ Fisher (2001), p. 73.
  7. ^ a b Fisher (2001), p. 76.
  8. ^ Fisher (2001), p. 77.
  9. ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), chpt 7
  10. ^ Wells, Accents of English, 1:224-225.
  11. ^ Gimson, Alfred Charles (2014), Cruttenden, Alan (ed.), Gimson's Pronunciation of English (8th ed.), Routledge, pp. 119–120, ISBN 978-1-4441-8309-2
  12. ^ Shorter Oxford English Dictionary
  13. ^ Wells, Accents of English, p. 490.
  14. ^ Wakelyn, Martin: "Rural dialects in England", in: Trudgill, Peter (1984): Language in the British Isles, p.77
  15. ^ a b Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English 3: Beyond the British Isles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. p. 629. ISBN 0-521-28541-0.
  16. ^ [1][dead link]
  17. ^ Wells, Accents of English, pp. 76, 221
  18. ^ Schneider, Edgar (2008). Varieties of English: The Americas and the Caribbean. Walter de Gruyter. p. 396.
  19. ^ McClear, Sheila (2 June 2010). "Why the classic Noo Yawk accent is fading away". New York Post. Retrieved 13 April 2013.
  20. ^ Trudgill, Peter (1984). Language in the British Isles. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-28409-7.
  21. ^ Milla, Robert McColl (2012). English Historical Sociolinguistics. Edinburgh University Press. pp. 25–26. ISBN 978-0-7486-4181-9.
  22. ^ Labov, Ash, and Boberg, 2006: pp. 47–48.
  23. ^ Trudgill, Peter (2010). Investigations in Sociohistorical Linguistics. Cambridge University Press.
  24. ^ Gick, Bryan. 1999. A gesture-based account of intrusive consonants in English. Phonology 16: 1, pp. 29–54. (pdf). Retrieved 12 November 2010.
  25. ^ Harris 2006: pp. 2–5.
  26. ^ Pollock et al., 1998.
  27. ^ http://www.atlas.mouton-content.com/secure/generalmodules/varieties/unit0000/virtualsession/vslessons/thomas.pdf] p. 16
  28. ^ Trudgill, Peter (2000). "Sociohistorical linguistics and dialect survival: a note on another Nova Scotian enclave". In Magnus Leung, ed. (ed.). Language Structure and Variation. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell International. p. 197. {{cite book}}: |editor= has generic name (help)
  29. ^ Brinton, Lauren and Leslie Arnovick. The English Language: A Linguistic History. Oxford University Press: Canada, 2006
  30. ^ a b c d e f Wells (1982)
  31. ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. 235
  32. ^ Wells, p. 287
  33. ^ Wells, p. 524
  34. ^ Wells (1982), p. 503
  35. ^ Dialectal variant of "horse"
  36. ^ Wells (1982), p. 504
  37. ^ Wells (1982), p. 544
  38. ^ Wells (1982), p. 577
  39. ^ Wells, p. 520
  40. ^ Dillard, Joey Lee (1980). Perspectives on American English. The Hague; New York: Walter de Gruyter. p. 53. ISBN 90-279-3367-7.
  41. ^ Wells, John C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-22919-7., pp. 136–37, 203–6, 234, 245–47, 339–40, 400, 419, 443, 576
  42. ^ Wells (1982), pp. 508-509
  43. ^ Wells (1982), p. 225
  44. ^ Upton, Clive; Eben Upton (2004). Oxford rhyming dictionary. Oxford University Press. p. 59. ISBN 0-19-280115-5.
  45. ^ Upton, Clive; Eben Upton (2004). Oxford rhyming dictionary. Oxford University Press. p. 60. ISBN 0-19-280115-5.

Works cited

  • Fisher, John Hurt (2001). "British and American, Continuity and Divergence". In Algeo, John (ed.). The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume VI: English in North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 59–85. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Labov, William; Ash, Sharon; Boberg, Charles (2006). The Atlas of North American English. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-11-016746-8.
  • Lass, Roger (1999). "Phonology and Morphology". In Lass, Roger (ed.). The Cambridge History of the English Language, Volume III: 1476–1776. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 56–186. ISBN 0-521-26476-6. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)
  • Pollock, Bailey, Berni, Fletcher, Hinton, Johnson, Roberts, & Weaver (17 March 2001). "Phonological Features of African American Vernacular English (AAVE)". Retrieved 8 November 2016.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Trudgill, Peter (1984). Language in the British Isles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Wells, J. C. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. {{cite book}}: Invalid |ref=harv (help)