Neuroticism: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Reverted to revision 794655657 by Alexbrn (talk): See Talk. (TW)
The removed source is from an academic textbook on psychology from September 2014. It is a valid source, so there is no excuse to remove it, even under the "5 years or newer" rule.
Line 176: Line 176:


==Sex differences==
==Sex differences==
Personality studies find that women score moderately higher than men on neuroticism, by approximately half of a standard deviation.<ref name="Lippa2010">{{cite journal|last1=Lippa|first1=Richard A.|title=Gender Differences in Personality and Interests: When, Where, and Why?|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass|volume=4|issue=11|year=2010|pages=1098–1110|issn=17519004|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x}}</ref>
Personality studies find that women score moderately higher than men on neuroticism, by approximately half of a standard deviation.<ref name="Lippa2010">{{cite journal|last1=Lippa|first1=Richard A.|title=Gender Differences in Personality and Interests: When, Where, and Why?|journal=Social and Personality Psychology Compass|volume=4|issue=11|year=2010|pages=1098–1110|issn=17519004|doi=10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x}}</ref><ref name="Schmitt2015">{{cite journal|last1=Schmitt|first1=David P.|title=The Evolution of Culturally-Variable Sex Differences: Men and Women Are Not Always Different, but When They Are…It Appears Not to Result from Patriarchy or Sex Role Socialization|year=2015|pages=221–256|issn=2197-9898|doi=10.1007/978-3-319-09384-0_11}}</ref>


== Evolutionary psychology ==
== Evolutionary psychology ==

Revision as of 14:55, 9 August 2017

Neuroticism is one of the Big Five higher-order personality traits in the study of psychology. Individuals who score high on neuroticism are more likely than average to be moody and to experience such feelings as anxiety, worry, fear, anger, frustration, envy, jealousy, guilt, depressed mood, and loneliness.[1][2] People who are neurotic respond worse to stressors and are more likely to interpret ordinary situations as threatening and minor frustrations as hopelessly difficult. They are often self-conscious and shy, and they may have trouble controlling urges and delaying gratification. High neuroticism indexes a risk constellation that exists prior to the development and onset of any of the "common mental disorders",[3][4] such as depression, phobia, panic disorder, other anxiety disorders, and substance use disorder—symptoms that traditionally have been called neuroses.[4][5][6][7][8]

Emotional stability

At the opposite end of the spectrum, individuals who score low in neuroticism are more emotionally stable and less reactive to stress. They tend to be calm, even-tempered, and less likely to feel tense or rattled. Although they are low in negative emotion, they are not necessarily high on positive emotion. Being high on positive emotion is an element of the independent trait of extraversion. Neurotic extraverts, for example, would experience high levels of both positive and negative emotional states, a kind of "emotional roller coaster". Individuals who score low on neuroticism (particularly those who are also high on extraversion) generally report more happiness and satisfaction with their lives.[9][10]

History

Galen of Pergamom popularized the idea that mixes of four bodily fluids or humours resulted in four personality types or temperaments. The melancholic personality type, which can be seen as the conceptual predecessor of neuroticism, was characterized by being mentally unbalanced, fearful, anxious, or sad. According to Hippocrates, it resulted from too much black bile.[11]

Measurement

Like other personality traits, neuroticism is typically viewed as a continuous dimension rather than a discrete state. Neuroticism test scores approximate a normal distribution given a large enough sample of people.

The extent of neuroticism is generally assessed using self-report measures, although peer-reports and third-party observation can also be used. Self-report measures are either lexical[1] or based on statements.[12] Deciding which measure of either type to use in research is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the study being undertaken.

Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect neurotic traits, such as anxiety, envy, jealousy, and moodiness, and are very space and time efficient for research purposes. Lewis Goldberg (1992)[13] developed a 20-word measure as part of his 100-word Big Five markers. Saucier (1994)[14] developed a briefer 8-word measure as part of his 40-word mini-markers. Thompson (2008)[1] systematically revised these measures to develop the International English Mini-Markers which has superior validity and reliability in populations both within and outside North America. Internal consistency reliability of the International English Mini-Markers for the Neuroticism (emotional stability) measure for native English-speakers is reported as .84, and that for non-native English-speakers is .77.[1]

Statement measures tend to comprise more words, and hence consume more research instrument space, than lexical measures. Respondents are asked the extent to which they, for example, "Remain calm under pressure", or "Have frequent mood swings".[12] While some statement-based measures of neuroticism have similarly acceptable psychometric properties in North American populations to lexical measures, their generally emic development makes them less suited to use in other populations.[15] For instance, statements in colloquial North American English like "Seldom feel blue" and "Am often down in the dumps" are sometimes hard for non-native English-speakers to understand.

Neuroticism has also been studied from the perspective of Gray's biopsychological theory of personality, using a scale that measures personality along two dimensions: the behavioural inhibition system (BIS) and the behavioural activation system (BAS).[16][17] The BIS is thought to be related to sensitivity to punishment as well as avoidance motivation, while the BAS is thought to be related to sensitivity to reward as well as approach motivation. Neuroticism has been found to be positively correlated with the BIS scale, and negatively correlated with the BAS scale.[18][19]

Life course development

In children and adolescents, psychologists speak of temperamental negative affectivity that, during adolescence, develops into the neuroticism personality domain.[20][21] Mean neuroticism levels change throughout the lifespan as a function of personality maturation and social roles,[22][23] but also the expression of new genes.[24] Generally speaking, the influence of environments on neuroticism increases over the lifespan,[24] although people probably select and evoke experiences based on their neuroticism levels.[25][21] It has been shown that people change in neuroticism after positive or negative life experiences.[26][25]

Psychopathology

Research has found that a wide range of clinical mental disorders are associated with elevated levels of neuroticism compared to levels in the general population.[4][27][28][29] Longitudinal studies indicate that high neuroticism is also predictive for the development of anxiety, depression, substance abuse, psychosis, schizophrenia, and non-specific mental distress;[3] also after adjustment for these elevated baseline symptoms and psychiatric history.[3]

Mood disorders

Disorders associated with elevated neuroticism include mood disorders, such as depression and bipolar disorder, anxiety disorders, eating disorders, schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder, dissociative identity disorder, and hypochondriasis. Mood disorders tend to have a much larger association with neuroticism than most other disorders.[3][4] A DSM-5 diagnosis distinguishes an episode (or "state") of depression from the habitual (or "trait") depressive symptoms someone can experience as part of neuroticism.[30]

Personality disorders

Personality disorders as listed in DSM-IV in general tend to be associated with elevated neuroticism.[4][31] A meta-analysis found that borderline, paranoid, schizotypal, avoidant, and dependent personality disorders were each associated with substantial levels of neuroticism (correlations ranging from .28 to .49).[citation needed] The remaining personality disorders had either modest positive or non-significant (in the case of narcissistic and histrionic) associations with neuroticism.

Neuropsychology

Neuroticism appears to be related to physiological differences in the brain. Hans Eysenck theorized that neuroticism is a function of activity in the limbic system, and his research suggests that people who score highly on measures of neuroticism have a more reactive sympathetic nervous system, and are more sensitive to environmental stimulation.[32] Behavioral genetics researchers have found that a significant portion of the variability on measures of neuroticism can be attributed to genetic factors.[33]

A study with positron emission tomography has found that healthy subjects that score high on the NEO PI-R neuroticism dimension tend to have high altanserin binding in the frontolimbic region of the brain—an indication that these subjects tend to have more of the 5-HT2A receptor in that location.[34] Another study has found that healthy subjects with a high neuroticism score tend to have higher DASB binding in the thalamus; DASB is a ligand that binds to the serotonin transporter protein.[35]

Another neuroimaging study using magnetic resonance imaging to measure brain volume found that the brain volume was negatively correlated to NEO PI-R neuroticism when correcting for possible effects of intracranial volume, sex, and age.[36]

Other studies have associated neuroticism with genetic variations, e.g. with 5-HTTLPR—a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene.[37] However, not all studies find such an association.[38] A genome-wide association study (GWA study) has associated single-nucleotide polymorphisms in the MDGA2 gene with neuroticism,[39] however the effect sizes were small. Another GWA study gave some evidence that the rs362584 polymorphism in the SNAP25 gene was associated with neuroticism.[40]

A 2008 experiment investigated the neurophysiological responses to uncertainty (which individuals high in neuroticism find aversive) using an event-related potential framework. Participants received positive, negative and uncertain feedback on a task while the feedback-related negativity (FRN), an evoked potential that peaks approximately 250 ms after the receipt of feedback information, was measured. For all participants, it was found that a larger FRN occurred after negative feedback than after positive feedback. However, for participants high on neuroticism, uncertain feedback resulted in a larger neural response than did negative feedback.[41]

A 2009 study has found that higher neuroticism is associated with greater loss of brain volume with increasing age.[42]

Mental-noise hypothesis

Studies have found that the mean reaction times will not differ between individuals high in neuroticism and those low in neuroticism, but that, with individuals high in neuroticism, there is considerably more trial-to-trial variability in performance reflected in reaction time standard deviations. In other words, on some trials neurotic individuals are faster than average, and on others they are slower than average. It has been suggested that this variability reflects noise in the individual's information processing systems or instability of basic cognitive operations (such as regulation processes), and further that this noise originates from two sources: mental preoccupations and reactivity processes.[43]

Flehmig et al. (2007) studied mental noise in terms of everyday behaviours using the Cognitive Failures Questionnaire, which is a self-report measure of the frequency of slips and lapses of attention. A "slip" is an error by commission, and a "lapse" is an error by omission. This scale was correlated with two well-known measures of neuroticism, the BIS/BAS scale and the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. Results indicated that the CFQ-UA (Cognitive Failures Questionnaire- Unintended Activation) subscale was most strongly correlated with neuroticism (r = .40)[clarification needed] and explained the most variance (16%) compared to overall CFQ scores, which only explained 7%. The authors interpret these findings as suggesting that mental noise is "highly specific in nature" as it is related most strongly to attention slips triggered endogenously by associative memory. In other words, this may suggest that mental noise is mostly task-irrelevant cognitions such as worries and preoccupations.[44]

Sex differences

Personality studies find that women score moderately higher than men on neuroticism, by approximately half of a standard deviation.[45][46]

Evolutionary psychology

The theory of evolution may also explain differences in personality.[47] For example, one of the evolutionary approaches to depression focuses on neuroticism.[48] A moderate amount of neuroticism may provide benefits, such as increased drive and productivity, due to greater sensitivity to negative outcomes.[47] However, too much may reduce fitness by producing, for example, recurring depressions. Thus, evolution will select for an optimal amount and most people will have neuroticism near this optimum. However, because neuroticism likely has a normal distribution in the population, a minority will be highly neurotic.[48]

Terror Management Theory

According to Terror Management Theory (TMT) neuroticism is primarily caused by insufficient anxiety buffers against unconscious death anxiety. These buffers consist of

1) Cultural worldviews that impart life with a sense of enduring meaning, such as social continuity beyond one's death, future legacy and afterlife beliefs, and

2) A sense of personal value or self-esteem in the context of this cultural worldview or enduring meaning.

While TMT agrees with standard Evolutionary Psychology accounts that the roots of neuroticism in Homo Sapiens or its ancestors are likely in adaptive sensitivities to negative outcomes, it posits that once Homo Sapiens achieved a higher level of self-awareness, neuroticism increased enormously, becoming largely a spandrel, a non-adaptive byproduct of our adaptive intelligence, which resulted in a crippling awareness of death that threatened to undermine other adaptive functions. This overblown anxiety thus needed to be buffered via intelligently creative, but largely fictitious and arbitrary notions of cultural meaning and personal value. Since highly religious or supernatural conceptions of the world provide "cosmic" personal significance and literal immortality, they are deemed to offer the most efficient buffers against death anxiety and neuroticism. Thus, historically, the shift to more materialistic and secular cultures - starting in the neolithic, and culminating in the industrial revolution, is deemed to have increased neuroticism.[49]

Core self-evaluations

Neuroticism has been included as one of the four dimensions that comprise core self-evaluations, one's fundamental appraisal of oneself, along with locus of control, self-efficacy, and self-esteem.[50] The concept of core self-evaluations was first examined by Judge, Locke, and Durham (1997),[50] and since then evidence has been found to suggest these have the ability to predict several work outcomes, specifically, job satisfaction and job performance.[50][51][52][53][54]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d Thompson, E.R. (October 2008). "Development and Validation of an International English Big-Five Mini-Markers". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542–548. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013.
  2. ^ G. Matthews and Ian J. Deary (1998). Personality traits. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  3. ^ a b c d Jeronimus B.F.; Kotov, R.; Riese, H.; Ormel, J. (2016). "Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants". Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883–2906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID 27523506.
  4. ^ a b c d e Ormel J.; Jeronimus, B.F.; Kotov, M.; Riese, H.; Bos, E.H.; Hankin, B. (2013). "Neuroticism and common mental disorders: Meaning and utility of a complex relationship". Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686–697. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC 4382368. PMID 23702592.
  5. ^ Hettema, J. M.; Neale, M. C.; Myers, J. M.; Prescott, C. A.; Kendler, K. S. (2006). "A population-based twin study of the relationship between neuroticism and internalizing disorders". American Journal of Psychiatry. 163 (5): 857–864. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.5.857.
  6. ^ "Personality correlates of male sexual arousal and behavior". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 5: 149–156. doi:10.1007/BF01541871.
  7. ^ Carducci, Bernardo J (2009-02-20). "The Psychology of Personality: Viewpoints, Research, and Applications". ISBN 978-1-4051-3635-8. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  8. ^ Leese, Peter (2014). Shell Shock: Traumatic Neurosis and the British Soldiers of the First World War. Palgrave MacMillan. ISBN 978-1-137-45337-2. Retrieved 2014-09-30.
  9. ^ Passer, Michael W.; Smith, Ronald E. (2009). Psychology: the science of mind and behaviour. McGraw-Hill Higher Education. ISBN 978-0-07-711836-5.
  10. ^ De Neve, K., Cooper, H. (1998). "The happy personality: A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being". Psychological Bulletin. 124 (2): 197–229. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197. PMID 9747186.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ Berrios, G. E. (1988). "Melancholia and depression during the 19th century: A conceptual history". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 153 (3): 298–304. doi:10.1192/bjp.153.3.298. PMID 3074848.
  12. ^ a b Goldberg, L.R.; Johnson, JA; Eber, HW; et al. (2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures". Journal of Research in Personality. 40 (1): 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007.
  13. ^ Goldberg, L.R. (1992). "The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure". Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26–42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26.
  14. ^ Saucier, G (1994). "Mini-Markers – a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers". Journal of Personality Assessment. 63 (3): 506–516. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8. PMID 7844738.
  15. ^ Piedmont, R.L.; Chae, J.H. (1997). "Cross-cultural generalizability of the five-factor model of personality - Development and validation of the NEO PI-R for Koreans". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 28 (2): 131–155. doi:10.1177/0022022197282001.
  16. ^ Gray, J.A. (1981). A critique of Eysenck's theory of personality, In H.J. Eysenck (Ed.) A model for personality (pp 246–276)
  17. ^ Gray, J.A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hippocampal system.
  18. ^ Boksema, M.A.S, Topsa, M., Westera, A.E., Meijmana, T.F. & Lorist, M.M. (June 2006). "Error-related ERP components and individual differences in punishment and reward sensitivity". Brain Research. 1101 (1): 92–101. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.05.004. PMID 16784728.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  19. ^ Carver, C.S.; White, T.L. (1994). "Behavioral Inhibition, Behavioral Activation, and Affective Responses to Impending Reward and Punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (2): 319–333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |lastauthoramp= ignored (|name-list-style= suggested) (help)
  20. ^ McAdams, D.P.; Olson, B.D. (2010). "Personality Development: Continuity and Change Over the Life Course". Annual Review of Psychology. 61: 517–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100507. PMID 19534589.
  21. ^ a b Caspi, A.; Shiner, R. (2011). Temperament and Personality, in Rutter's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (5th ed). Malden: Blackwell Publishing Limited. p. 182. ISBN 9781444300895.
  22. ^ Roberts, B.W., Walton, K.E., Viechtbauer, W. (2006). "Patterns of Mean-Level Change in Personality Traits Across the Life Course: A Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. PMID 16435954.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  23. ^ McCrae, R.R. (1999). "Age Differences in Personality Across the Adult Life Span: Parallels in Five Cultures". Developmental Psychology. 35 (2): 466–477. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.466. PMID 10082017. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |displayauthors= ignored (|display-authors= suggested) (help)
  24. ^ a b Briley, D. A., Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2014). "Genetic and environmental continuity in personality development: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 140 (5): 1303–31. doi:10.1037/a0037091. PMC 4152379. PMID 24956122.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  25. ^ a b Jeronimus; et al. (2014). "Mutual Reinforcement Between Neuroticism and Life Experiences: A Five-Wave, 16-Year Study to Test Reciprocal Causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  26. ^ Riese, H. (2014). "Timing of Stressful Life Events Affects Stability and Change of Neuroticism". European Journal of Personality. 28 (2): 193–200. doi:10.1002/per.1929. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |displayauthors= ignored (|display-authors= suggested) (help)
  27. ^ Malouff, J.M., Thorsteinsson, E.B., & Schutte N.S. (2005). "The relationship between the five factor model of personality and symptoms of clinical disorders". Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 27 (2): 101–114. doi:10.1007/s10862-005-5384-y.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ Jeronimus, B.F., Ormel, J., Aleman, A., Penninx, B.W.J.H., Riese, H. (2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  29. ^ Kotov; et al. (2010). "Linking "big" personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768–821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID 20804236.
  30. ^ Riese; et al. (2016). "Don't throw the baby out with the bathwater: Depressive traits are part and parcel of neuroticism". NeuroImage. 125: 1103. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.012. PMID 26551260.
  31. ^ Saulsman, L.M.; Page, A.C. (2004). "The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review". Clinical Psychology Review. 23 (8): 1055–1085. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2002.09.001. PMID 14729423. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |lastauthoramp= ignored (|name-list-style= suggested) (help)
  32. ^ Hans Jürgen Eysenck and Michael W. Eysenck (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. Perspectives on individual differences. Plenum Press (Springer). ISBN 0-306-41844-4.
  33. ^ Viken RJ, Rose RJ, Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M (April 1994). "A developmental genetic analysis of adult personality: extraversion and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 66 (4): 722–30. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.722. PMID 8189349.
  34. ^ Vibe G. Frøkjær; Erik L. Mortensen; Finn Årup Nielsen; Steven Haugbøl; Lars H. Pinborg; Karen H. Adams; Claus Svarer; Steen G. Hasselbalch; Søren Holm; Olaf B. Paulson; Gitte Moos Knudsen (2007). "Frontolimbic Serotonin 2A Receptor Binding in Healthy Subjects Is Associated with Personality Risk Factors for Affective Disorder". Biological Psychiatry. 63 (6): 569–76. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.009. PMID 17884017.
  35. ^ Akihiro Takano; Ryosuke Arakawaa; Mika Hayashia; Hidehiko Takahashia; Hiroshi Itoa; Tetsuya Suhara (September 2007). "Relationship between neuroticism personality trait and serotonin transporter binding". Biological Psychiatry. 62 (6): 588–592. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.11.007. PMID 17336939.
  36. ^ Brian Knutsona; Reza Momenan; Robert R. Rawlings; Grace W. Fong; Daniel Hommer (November 2001). "Negative association of neuroticism with brain volume ratio in healthy humans". Biological Psychiatry. 50 (9): 685–690. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01220-3. PMID 11704075.
  37. ^ Klaus-Peter Lesch, D. Bengel, A. Heils, S. Z. Sabol, B. D. Greenberg, S. Petri, J. Benjamin, C. R. Muller, D. H. Hamer, & Dennis L. Murphy (November 1996). "Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region". Science. 274 (5292): 1527–31. doi:10.1126/science.274.5292.1527. PMID 8929413.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  38. ^ A. F. Jorm; A. S. Henderson; P. A. Jacomb; H. Christensen; A. E. Korten; B. Rodgers; X. Tan; S. Easteal (September 1998). "An association study of a functional polymorphism of the serotonin transporter gene with personality and psychiatric symptoms". Molecular Psychiatry. 3 (5): 449–51. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4000424. PMID 9774781.
  39. ^ E. J. van den Oord, P. H. Kuo, A. M. Hartmann, B. T. Webb, H. J. Möller, J. M. Hettema, I. Giegling, J. Bukszár, D. Rujescu (September 2008). "Genomewide Association Analysis Followed by a Replication Study Implicates a Novel Candidate Gene for Neuroticism". Archives of General Psychiatry. 65 (9): 1062–1071. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.9.1062. PMID 18762592.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  40. ^ A. Terracciano, S Sanna, M. Uda, B. Deiana, G. Usala, F. Busonero, A. Maschio, M. Scally, N. Patriciu, W.-M. Chen, M. A. Distel, E. P. Slagboom, D. I. Boomsma, S. Villafuerte, E. S. liwerska, M. Burmeister, N. Amin, A. C. J. W. Janssens, C. M. van Duijn, D. Schlessinger, G. R. Abecasis and P. T. Costa Jr (October 2008). "Genome-wide association scan for five major dimensions of personality". Molecular Psychiatry. 15 (6): 647–56. doi:10.1038/mp.2008.113. PMC 2874623. PMID 18957941.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  41. ^ Hirsh J. B.; Inzlicht M. (2008). "The devil you know: Neuroticism predicts neural response to uncertainty". Psychological Science. 19 (10): 962–967. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02183.x. PMID 19000202.
  42. ^ Jackson, J.; Balota, D.; Head, D. (2009). "Exploring the relationship between personality and regional brain volume in healthy aging". Neurobiol Aging. 32 (12): 2162–2171. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.12.009. ISSN 0197-4580. PMC 2891197. PMID 20036035. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |laysummary= ignored (help)
  43. ^ Robinson, M.D; Tamir, M. (2006). "Neuroticism as mental noise: a relation between neuroticism and reaction time standard deviations". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 89 (1): 107–114. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.107. PMID 16060749. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |lastauthoramp= ignored (|name-list-style= suggested) (help)
  44. ^ Flehmig, H.C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R., & Westhoff, K. (2007). "Neuroticism and the mental noise hypothesis: Relationships to lapses of attention and slips of action in everyday life". Psychology Science. 49 (4): 343–360.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  45. ^ Lippa, Richard A. (2010). "Gender Differences in Personality and Interests: When, Where, and Why?". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 4 (11): 1098–1110. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00320.x. ISSN 1751-9004.
  46. ^ Schmitt, David P. (2015). "The Evolution of Culturally-Variable Sex Differences: Men and Women Are Not Always Different, but When They Are…It Appears Not to Result from Patriarchy or Sex Role Socialization": 221–256. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-09384-0_11. ISSN 2197-9898. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  47. ^ a b Buss, D.M. (1991). "Evolutionary personality psychology". Annual Review of Psychology. 42: 459–491. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.42.1.459.
  48. ^ a b Allen, N.; Badcock, P. (2006). "Darwinian models of depression: A review of evolutionary accounts of mood and mood disorders". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 30 (5): 815–826. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.007. PMID 16647176.
  49. ^ Thirty Years of Terror Management Theory Tom Pyszczynski, Sheldon Solomon, Jeff Greenberg https://www.researchgate.net/publication/289309102_Thirty_Years_of_Terror_Management_Theory
  50. ^ a b c Judge T. A.; Locke E. A.; Durham C. C. (1997). "The dispositional causes of job satisfaction: A core evaluations approach". Research in Organizational Behavior. 19: 151–188.
  51. ^ Bono J. E.; Judge T. A. (2003). "Core self-evaluations: A review of the trait and its role in job satisfaction and job performance". European Journal of Personality. 17 (Suppl 1): S5–S18. doi:10.1002/per.48.
  52. ^ Dormann C.; Fay D.; Zapf D.; Frese M. (2006). "A state-trait analysis of job satisfaction: On the effect of core self-evaluations". Applied Psychology: An International Review. 55 (1): 27–51. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00227.x.
  53. ^ Judge T. A.; Locke E. A.; Durham C. C.; Kluger A. N. (1998). "Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: The role of core evaluations". Journal of Applied Psychology. 83 (1): 17–34. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.17. PMID 9494439.
  54. ^ Judge T. A.; Bono J. E. (2001). "Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job performance: A meta-analysis". Journal of Applied Psychology. 86 (1): 80–92. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.1.80. PMID 11302235.