Empathy
Empathy is the capacity to recognize emotions that are being experienced by another sentient or fictional being. One may need to have a certain amount of empathy before being able to experience accurate sympathy or compassion. The English word was coined in 1909 by the psychologist Edward B. Titchener in an attempt to translate the German word "Einfühlungsvermögen", a new phenomenon explored at the end of 19th century mainly by philosopher Theodor Lipps. It was later re-translated into the German language as "Empathie", and is still in use there.[1]
Etymology
The English word is derived from the Greek word ἐμπάθεια (empatheia), "physical affection, passion, partiality" which comes from ἐν (en), "in, at" + πάθος (pathos), "passion" or "suffering".[2] The term was adapted by Hermann Lotze and Robert Vischer to create the German word Einfühlung ("feeling into"), which was translated by Edward B. Titchener into the English term empathy.[3]
Alexithymia, from the Ancient Greek words ἀλέξω (alexo, "repel") and θυμός (thumos), literally meaning "pushing away emotions", is a term to describe a state of deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing emotions in oneself.[4][5]
Note that in modern Greek the word empathy (εμπάθεια) translates as "hatred", "Loathing","malevolence" and "spitefulness" (a situation of causing passion, rather than mutual relation to one's passion); ενσυναίσθηση is the correct modern equivalent of empathy.[citation needed]
==Defini
Empathy has many different definitions that encompass a broad range of emotional states, from caring for other people and having a desire to help them, to experiencing emotions that match another person's emotions, to knowing what the other person is thinking or feeling, to blurring the line between self and other.[6]
Since empathy involves understanding the emotional states of other people, the way it is characterized is derivative of the way emotions themselves are characterized. If, for example, emotions are taken to be centrally characterized by bodily feelings, then grasping the bodily feelings of another will be central to empathy. On the other hand, if emotions are more centrally characterized by a combination of beliefs and desires, then grasping these beliefs and desires will be more essential to empathy. The ability to imagine oneself as another person is a sophisticated imaginative process. However, the basic capacity to recognize emotions is probably innate[7] and may be achieved unconsciously. Yet it can be trained[8] and achieved with various degrees of intensity or accuracy.
Empathy necessarily has a "more or less" quality. The paradigm case of an empathic interaction, however, involves a person communicating an accurate recognition of the significance of another person's ongoing intentional actions, associated emotional states, and personal characteristics in a manner that the recognized person can tolerate. Recognitions that are both accurate and tolerable are central features of empathy.[9][10]
The human capacity to recognize the bodily feelings of another is related to one's imitative capacities and seems to be grounded in an innate capacity to associate the bodily movements and facial expressions one sees in another with the proprioceptive feelings of producing those corresponding movements or expressions oneself.[11] Humans seem to make the same immediate connection between the tone of voice and other vocal expressions and inner feeling.
Empathy is distinct from sympathy, pity, and emotional contagion.[12] Sympathy or empathic concern is the feeling of compassion or concern for another, the wish to see them better off or happier. Pity is feeling that another is in trouble and in need of help as they cannot fix their problems themselves, often described as "feeling sorry" for someone. Emotional contagion is when a person (especially an infant or a member of a mob) imitatively "catches" the emotions that others are showing without necessarily recognizing this is happening.[13]
Types of empathy
Affective and cognitive empathy
Empathy can be divided into two major components:[14]
- Affective empathy, also called emotional empathy:[15] the capacity to respond with an appropriate emotion to another's mental states.[14] Our ability to empathize emotionally is supposed to be based on emotional contagion:[15] being affected by another's emotional or arousal state.[16]
- Cognitive empathy: the capacity to understand another's perspective or mental state.[14][17] The terms cognitive empathy and theory of mind are often used synonymously, but due to a lack of studies comparing theory of mind with types of empathy, it is unclear whether these are equivalent.[18]
Although science has not yet agreed upon a precise definition of these constructs, there is consensus about this distinction.[19] There is a difference in disturbance between affective and cognitive empathy in different psychiatric disorders. Psychopathy, schizophrenia, depersonalization, and narcissism are characterized by impairments in affective empathy but not in cognitive empathy, whereas bipolar disorder, borderline traits, and, by some accounts,[20][21] autism are associated with deficits in cognitive empathy but not in affective empathy.[19] Even in people without mental disorders, the balance between affective and cognitive empathy varies.[19] A meta-analysis of recent fMRI studies of empathy confirmed that different brain areas are activated during affective–perceptual empathy and cognitive–evaluative empathy.[22] Also a study with patients with different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.[15] Specifically, the inferior frontal gyrus appears to be responsible for emotional empathy, and the ventromedial prefrontal gyrus seems to mediate cognitive empathy.[15]
Affective empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[14][23]
- Empathic concern: sympathy and compassion for others in response to their suffering.[14][20][24]
- Personal distress: self-centered feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to another's suffering.[14][20][24]
There is no consensus regarding whether personal distress is a basic form of empathy or instead does not constitute empathy.[20] There may be a developmental aspect to this subdivision. Infants respond to the distress of others by getting distressed themselves; only when they are 2 years old do they start to respond in other-oriented ways, trying to help, comfort and share.[20]
Cognitive empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[14][23]
- Perspective taking: the tendency to spontaneously adopt others' psychological perspectives.[14]
- Fantasy: the tendency to identify with fictional characters.[14]
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is the only published measurement tool to date that accounts for a multi-dimensional assessment of empathy. It comprises a self-report questionnaire of 28 items, divided into four 7-item scales covering the above subdivisions of affective and cognitive empathy.[14][23]
Development
By the age of two years, children normally begin to display the fundamental behaviors of empathy by having an emotional response that corresponds with another person's emotional state.[25] Even earlier, at one year of age, infants have some rudiments of empathy, in the sense that they understand that, just like their own actions, other people's actions have goals.[26][27][28] Sometimes, toddlers will comfort others or show concern for them at as early an age as two. Also during the second year, toddlers will play games of falsehood or "pretend" in an effort to fool others, and this requires that the child know what others believe before he or she can manipulate those beliefs.[29]
According to researchers at the University of Chicago who used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), children between the ages of 7 and 12 years appear to be naturally inclined to feel empathy for others in pain. Their findings[30] are consistent with previous fMRI studies of pain empathy with adults. The research also found additional aspects of the brain were activated when youngsters saw another person intentionally hurt by another individual, including regions involved in moral reasoning.[31]
Despite being able to show some signs of empathy, such as attempting to comfort a crying baby, from as early as 18 months to two years, most children do not show a fully fledged theory of mind until around the age of four.[32] Theory of mind involves the ability to understand that other people may have beliefs that are different from one's own, and is thought to involve the cognitive component of empathy.[17] Children usually become capable of passing "false belief" tasks, considered to be a test for a theory of mind, around the age of four. Individuals with autism often find using a theory of mind very difficult (e.g. Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith, 1988; the Sally-Anne test).
Empathetic maturity is a cognitive structural theory developed at the Yale University School of Nursing and addresses how adults conceive or understand the personhood of patients. The theory, first applied to nurses and since applied to other professions, postulates three levels that have the properties of cognitive structures. The third and highest level is held to be a meta-ethical theory of the moral structure of care. Those adults operating with level-III understanding synthesize systems of justice and care-based ethics.[33]
Neurological basis
Research in recent years has focused on possible brain processes underlying the experience of empathy. For instance, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been employed to investigate the functional anatomy of empathy.[34][35] These studies have shown that observing another person's emotional state activates parts of the neuronal network involved in processing that same state in oneself, whether it is disgust,[36] touch,[37][38] or pain.[39][40][41][42] The study of the neural underpinnings of empathy has received increased interest following the target paper published by Preston and Frans de Waal,[43] following the discovery of mirror neurons in monkeys that fire both when the creature watches another perform an action as well as when they themselves perform it.
In their paper, they argued that attended perception of the object's state automatically activates neural representations, and that this activation automatically primes or generates the associated autonomic and somatic responses (idea of perception-action-coupling[44]), unless inhibited. This mechanism is similar to the common coding theory between perception and action. Another recent study provides evidence of separate neural pathways activating reciprocal suppression in different regions of the brain associated with the performance of "social" and "mechanical" tasks. These findings suggest that the cognition associated with reasoning about the "state of another person's mind" and "causal/mechanical properties of inanimate objects" are neurally suppressed from occurring at the same time.[45][46]
Anger and distress
Anger
Empathic anger is an emotion, a form of empathic distress.[47] Empathic anger is felt in a situation where someone else is being hurt by another person or thing. It is possible to see this form of anger as a pro-social emotion.[citation needed]
Empathic anger has direct effects on both helping and punishing desires. Empathic anger can be divided into two sub-categories: trait empathic anger and state empathic anger.[48]
The relationship between empathy and anger response towards another person has also been investigated, with two studies basically finding that the higher a person's perspective taking ability, the less angry they were in response to a provocation. Empathic concern did not, however, significantly predict anger response, and higher personal distress was associated with increased anger.[49][50]
Distress
Empathic distress is feeling the perceived pain of another person. This feeling can be transformed into empathic anger, feelings of injustice, or guilt. These emotions can be perceived as pro-social, and some say they can be seen as motives for moral behavior.[47]
Atypical empathic response
Atypical empathic responses are found in people on the autism spectrum, those with particular personality disorders such as borderline personality disorder, psychopathy, narcissistic personality disorder, and schizoid personality disorder, in addition to people with conduct disorder,[51] schizophrenia or bipolar disorder,[19] and those experiencing depersonalization.[52]
Autism spectrum
The interaction between empathy and the autism spectrum is a complex and ongoing field of research. Several different factors are proposed to be at play here.
Alexithymia
A study of high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorders found an increased prevalence of alexithymia,[53] a personality construct characterized by the inability to recognize and articulate emotional arousal in oneself or others.[53][54][55] Based on fMRI studies, alexithymia is responsible for a lack of empathy.[56] The lack of empathic attunement inherent to alexithymic states may reduce quality[57] and satisfaction[58] of relationships. Recently, a study has shown that high-functioning adults with autism appear to have a range of responses to music similar to that of neurotypical individuals, including the deliberate use of music for mood management. Clinical treatment of alexithymia could involve using a simple associative learning process between musically induced emotions and their cognitive correlates.[59] A study has suggested that the empathy deficits associated with the autism spectrum may be due to significant comorbidity between alexithymia and autism spectrum conditions rather than a result of social impairment.[60]
Mirror neuron activity
One study found that, relative to typically developing children, high-functioning children with autism showed reduced mirror neuron activity in the brain's inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) while imitating and observing emotional expressions.[61] EEG evidence revealed that there was significantly greater mu suppression in the sensorimotor cortex of autistic individuals. Activity in this area was inversely related to symptom severity in the social domain, suggesting that a dysfunctional mirror neuron system may underlie social and communication deficits observed in autism, including impaired theory of mind and empathy.[62] The mirror neuron system is essential for emotional empathy.[15]
Theory of mind
Previous studies have suggested that autistic individuals have impaired theory of mind. Theory of mind is the ability to understand the perspectives of others.[14] The terms cognitive empathy and theory of mind are often used synonymously, but due to a lack of studies comparing theory of mind with types of empathy, it is unclear whether these are equivalent.[14] Theory of mind relies on structures of the temporal lobe and the pre-frontal cortex, and empathy, i.e. the ability to share the feelings of others, relies on the sensorimotor cortices as well as limbic and para-limbic structures.[citation needed] Francesca Happe showed that autistic children who demonstrate a lack of theory of mind lack it for their self as well as for others.[63][dead link][citation needed] The lack of clear distinctions between theory of mind and empathy may have resulted in an incomplete understanding of the empathic abilities of those with Asperger syndrome; many reports on the empathic deficits of individuals with Asperger syndrome are actually based on impairments in theory of mind.[14][64][65]
Cognitive and affective empathy
Studies have found that individuals on the autism spectrum self-report lower levels of empathic concern, show less or absent comforting responses toward someone who is suffering, and report equal or higher levels of personal distress compared to controls.[20] The combination of reduced empathic concern and increased personal distress may lead to the overall reduction of empathy in those on the autism spectrum.[20] Professor Simon Baron-Cohen suggests that those with classic autism often lack both cognitive and affective empathy.[21] Research also suggests that people with Asperger syndrome may have problems understanding others' perspectives in terms of theory of mind, but on average demonstrate equal empathic concern as and higher personal distress than controls.[14] The generally heightened personal distress in those with autism spectrum conditions has been offered as an explanation to the claim that at least some people with autism would appear to have heightened emotional empathy,[20][66] although emotional empathy depends on mirror neuron activity, which (as described previously) has been found to be reduced in those with autism, and empathy in people on the autism spectrum is generally reduced.[15][20]
Empathizing–systemizing theory
The empathizing–systemizing (E-S) theory suggests that people may be classified on the basis of their capabilities along two independent dimensions, empathizing (E) and systemizing (S). These capabilities may be inferred through tests that measure someone's Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Five different "brain types" can be observed among the population based on the scores, which should correlate with differences at the neural level. In the E-S theory, autism and Asperger syndrome are associated with below-average empathy and average or above-average systemizing. The E-S theory has been extended into the Extreme Male Brain theory, which suggests that people with an autism spectrum condition are more likely to have an "Extreme Type S" brain type, corresponding with above-average systemizing but challenged empathy (see the next section).[67]
Sex differences and autism
It has been shown that males are generally less empathetic than females.[67][68] The Extreme Male Brain (EMB) theory proposes that individuals on the autistic spectrum are characterized by impairments in empathy due to sex differences in the brain: specifically, people with autism spectrum conditions show an exaggerated male profile. A study showed that some aspects of autistic neuroanatomy seem to be extremes of typical male neuroanatomy, which may be influenced by elevated levels of fetal testosterone rather than gender itself.[67][69] Another study involving brain scans of 120 men and women suggested that autism affects male and female brains differently: females with autism had brains that appeared to be closer to those of non-autistic males than females, yet the same kind of difference was not observed in males with autism.[70]
Personality disorders
Atypical empathy is a trait of some personality disorders, including borderline personality disorder, histrionic personality disorder, psychopathy, narcissistic personality disorder, and schizoid personality disorder.
Borderline personality disorder
Borderline personality disorder is characterized by extensive behavioral and interpersonal difficulties that arise from emotional and cognitive dysfunction.[71] Dysfunctional social and interpersonal behavior has been shown to play a crucial role in the emotionally intense way people with borderline personality disorder react.[72] While individuals with borderline personality disorder may show their emotions too much, several authors have suggested that they might have a compromised ability to reflect upon mental states (impaired cognitive empathy), as well as an impaired theory of mind.[72]
People with borderline personality disorder are very good at recognizing emotions in people's faces, suggesting increased empathic capacities.[73][74] It is, therefore, possible that impaired cognitive empathy (the capacity for understanding another person's experience and perspective) may account for borderline personality disorder individuals' tendency for interpersonal dysfunction, while "hyper-emotional empathy"[verification needed] may account for the emotional over-reactivity observed in these individuals.[72] One primary study confirmed that patients with borderline personality disorder were significantly impaired in cognitive empathy, yet there was no sign of impairment in affective empathy.[72]
Psychopathy
Psychopaths exhibit antisocial and aggressive behavior, as well as emotional and interpersonal deficits including shallow emotions and a lack of remorse and empathy.[75][76] Studies suggest psychopaths have atypical responses to distress cues (e.g. facial and vocal expressions of fear and sadness), including decreased activation of the fusiform and extrastriate cortical regions, which may partly account for impaired recognition of and reduced autonomic responsiveness to expressions of fear, and impairments of empathy.[77][78][79][80][81][82] If psychopaths are instructed to empathise with harmed individuals, their empathic reaction via the mirror system initiates the same way it does for controls, although it remains unclear whether the same overall empathic experience is achieved and whether it is possible to transform the empathy into the more spontaneous response most people have.[83] The underlying biological surfaces for processing expressions of happiness are functionally intact in psychopaths, although less responsive than those of controls.[77][78][79][82] Professor Simon Baron-Cohen suggests that, unlike the combination of both reduced cognitive and affective empathy often seen in those with classic autism, psychopaths are aware of the feelings of others when they hurt someone because their cognitive empathy is intact even if affective empathy is not.[21]
Narcissistic personality disorder
One diagnostic criterion of narcissistic personality disorder is a lack of empathy and an unwillingness to recognize or identify with the feelings and needs of others.[84]
Schizoid personality disorder
Characteristics of schizoid personality disorder include emotional coldness, detachment, and impaired affect corresponding with an inability to be empathetic and sensitive towards others.[85][86][87][88]
Conduct disorder
A study conducted by Jean Decety and colleagues at the University of Chicago demonstrated that subjects with aggressive conduct disorder elicit atypical empathic responses to viewing others in pain.[51] Subjects with conduct disorder were at least as responsive as controls to the pain of others, but unlike controls, subjects with conduct disorder showed strong and specific activation of the amygdala and ventral striatum (areas that enable a general arousing effect of reward), yet impaired activation of the neural regions involved in self-regulation and metacognition (including moral reasoning), in addition to diminished processing between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.[51]
Depersonalization
Lieutenant Colonel Dave Grossman, in his book On Killing, suggests that military training artificially creates depersonalization in soldiers, suppressing empathy and making it easier for them to kill other human beings.[52]
Practical issues
This section has an unclear citation style. (January 2013) |
The capacity to empathize is a revered trait in society.[14] Empathy is considered a motivating factor for unselfish, prosocial behavior,[89] whereas a lack of empathy is related to antisocial behavior.[14][90][91]
Proper empathic engagement helps an individual understand and anticipate the behavior of another. Apart from the automatic tendency to recognize the emotions of others, one may also deliberately engage in empathic reasoning. Two general methods have been identified here.[92] An individual may simulate fictitious versions of the beliefs, desires, character traits and context of another individual to see what emotional feelings it provokes. Or, an individual may simulate an emotional feeling and then access the environment for a suitable reason for the emotional feeling to be appropriate for that specific environment.[citation needed]
Some research suggests that people are more able and willing to empathize with those most similar to themselves. In particular, empathy increases with similarities in culture and living conditions. Empathy is more likely to occur between individuals whose interaction is more frequent. (See Levenson and Reuf 1997 and Hoffman 2000: 62). A measure of how well a person can infer the specific content of another person's thoughts and feelings has been developed by William Ickes (1997, 2003). Ickes and his colleagues have developed a video-based method to measure empathic accuracy and have used this method to study the empathic inaccuracy of maritally aggressive and abusive spouses, among other topics.[citation needed]
There are concerns that the empathiser's own emotional background may affect or distort what emotions they perceive in others (e.g. Goleman 1996: p. 104). Empathy is not a process that is likely to deliver certain judgments about the emotional states of others. It is a skill that is gradually developed throughout life, and which improves the more contact we have with the person with whom one empathizes. Accordingly, any knowledge gained of the emotions of the other must be revisable in light of further information.[citation needed]
Ethical issues
The extent to which a person's emotions are publicly observable, or mutually recognized as such has significant social consequences. Empathic recognition may or may not be welcomed or socially desirable. This is particularly the case where we recognize the emotions that someone has towards us during real time interactions. Based on a metaphorical affinity with touch, philosopher Edith Wyschogrod claims that the proximity entailed by empathy increases the potential vulnerability of either party.[93] The appropriate role of empathy in our dealings with others is highly dependent on the circumstances. For instance, Tania Singer claims that clinicians or caregivers must take care not to be too sensitive to the emotions of others, to over-invest their own emotions, at the risk of draining away their own resourcefulness.[94] Furthermore an awareness of the limitations of empathic accuracy is prudent in a caregiving situation.
Disciplinary approaches
Philosophy
Ethics
In his 2008 book, Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, philosopher Iain King presents two reasons why empathy is the "essence" or "DNA" of right and wrong. First, he argues that empathy uniquely has all the characteristics we can know about an ethical viewpoint[95] – including that it is "partly self-standing", and so provides a source of motivation that is partly within us and partly outside, as moral motivations seem to be.[96] This allows empathy-based judgements to have sufficiently distance from a personal opinion to count as "moral". His second argument is more practical: he argues, "Empathy for others really is the route to value in life", and so the means by which a selfish attitude can become a moral one.[96] By using empathy as the basis for a system of ethics, King is able to reconcile ethics based on consequences with virtue-ethics and act-based accounts of right and wrong.[97] His empathy-based system has been taken up by some Buddhists,[98] and is used to address some practical problems, such as when to tell lies,[99] and how to develop culturally-neutral rules for romance.
In the 2007 book The Ethics of Care and Empathy, philosopher Michael Slote introduces a theory of care-based ethics that is grounded in empathy. His claim is that moral motivation does, and should, stem from a basis of empathic response. He claims that our natural reaction to situations of moral significance are explained by empathy. He explains that the limits and obligations of empathy and in turn morality are natural. These natural obligations include a greater empathic, and moral obligation to family and friends, along with an account of temporal and physical distance. In situations of close temporal and physical distance, and with family or friends, our moral obligation seems stronger to us than with strangers at a distance naturally. Slote explains that this is due to empathy and our natural empathic ties. He further adds that actions are wrong if and only if they reflect or exhibit a deficiency of fully developed empathic concern for others on the part of the agent.[100]
Phenomenology
In phenomenology, empathy describes the experience of something from the other's viewpoint, without confusion between self and other. This draws on the sense of agency. In the most basic sense, this is the experience of the other's body and, in this sense, it is an experience of "my body over there". In most other respects, however, the experience is modified so that what is experienced is experienced as being the other's experience; in experiencing empathy, what is experienced is not "my" experience, even though I experience it. Empathy is also considered to be the condition of intersubjectivity and, as such, the source of the constitution of objectivity.[101]
History
Some postmodern historians such as Keith Jenkins in recent years have debated whether or not it is possible to empathise with people from the past. Jenkins argues that empathy only enjoys such a privileged position in the present because it corresponds harmoniously with the dominant liberal discourse of modern society and can be connected to John Stuart Mill's concept of reciprocal freedom. Jenkins argues the past is a foreign country and as we do not have access to the epistemological conditions of by gone ages we are unable to empathise.[102]
It is impossible to forecast the effect of empathy on the future.[citation needed] A past subject may take part in the present by the so-called historic present. If we watch from a fictitious past, can tell the present with the future tense, as it happens with the trick of the false prophecy. There is no way of telling the present with the means of the past.[103]
Evolution of empathy
An increasing number of studies in animal behavior and neuroscience claim that empathy is not restricted to humans, and is in fact as old as the mammals, or perhaps older. Examples include dolphins saving humans from drowning or from shark attacks. Professor Tom White suggests that reports of cetaceans having three times as many spindle cells — the nerve cells that convey empathy — in their brains as we do might mean these highly social animals have a great awareness of one another's feelings.[104]
A multitude of behaviors observed in primates, both in captivity and in the wild, and in particular in bonobos, which are reported as the most empathetic of all the primates.[105][106] A recent study has demonstrated prosocial behavior elicited by empathy in rodents.[107]
Rodents have been shown to demonstrate empathy for cagemates (but not strangers) in pain.[108] One of the most widely read studies on the evolution of empathy, which discusses a neural perception-action mechanism (PAM), is the one by Stephanie Preston and de Waal ([92]). This review postulates a bottom-up model of empathy that ties together all levels, from state matching to perspective-taking. For University of Chicago neurobiologist Jean Decety, [empathy] is not specific to humans. He argues that there is strong evidence that empathy has deep evolutionary, biochemical, and neurological underpinnings, and that even the most advanced forms of empathy in humans are built on more basic forms and remain connected to core mechanisms associated with affective communication, social attachment, and parental care.[109] Core neural circuits that are involved in empathy and caring include the brainstem, the amygdala, hypothalamus, basal ganglia, insula and orbitofrontal cortex.[110]
Psychotherapy
Heinz Kohut is the main introducer of the principle of empathy in psychoanalysis. His principle applies to the method of gathering unconscious material. The possibility of not applying the principle is granted in the cure, for instance when you must reckon with another principle, that of reality. Developing skills of empathy is often a central theme in the recovery process for drug addicts.[citation needed]
In evolutionary psychology, attempts at explaining pro-social behavior often mention the presence of empathy in the individual as a possible variable. Although exact motives behind complex social behaviors are difficult to distinguish, the "ability to put oneself in the shoes of another person and experience events and emotions the way that person experienced them" is the definitive factor for truly altruistic behavior according to Batson's empathy-altruism hypothesis. If empathy is not felt, social exchange (what's in it for me?) supersedes pure altruism, but if empathy is felt, an individual will help by actions or by word, regardless of whether it is in their self-interest to do so and even if the costs outweigh potential rewards.[111]
Education
An important target of the method Learning by teaching (LbT) is to train systematically and, in each lesson, teach empathy. Students have to transmit new content to their classmates, so they have to reflect continuously on the mental processes of the other students in the classroom. This way it is possible to develop step-by-step the students' feeling for group reactions and networking. Carl R. Rogers pioneered research in effective psychotherapy and teaching which espoused that empathy coupled with unconditional positive regard or caring for students and authenticity or congruence were the most important traits for a therapist or teacher to have. Other research and publications by Tausch, Aspy, Roebuck. Lyon, and meta-analyses by Cornelius-White, corroborated the importance of these person-centered traits.[112][113]
Business and management
In the 2009 book Wired to Care, strategy consultant Dev Patnaik argues that a major flaw in contemporary business practice is a lack of empathy inside large corporations. He states that lacking any sense of empathy, people inside companies struggle to make intuitive decisions and often get fooled into believing they understand their business if they have quantitative research to rely upon. Patnaik claims that the real opportunity for companies doing business in the 21st Century is to create a widely held sense of empathy for customers, pointing to Nike, Harley-Davidson, and IBM as examples of "Open Empathy Organizations". Such institutions, he claims, see new opportunities more quickly than competitors, adapt to change more easily, and create workplaces that offer employees a greater sense of mission in their jobs.[114] In the 2011 book The Empathy Factor, organizational consultant Marie Miyashiro similarly argues the value of bringing empathy to the workplace, and offers Nonviolent Communication as an effective mechanism for achieving this.[115] In studies by the Management Research Group, empathy was found to be the strongest predictor of ethical leadership behavior out of 22 competencies in its management model, and empathy was one of the three strongest predictors of senior executive effectiveness.[116]
Intercultural empathy
Intercultural empathy is the ability to perceive the world as it is perceived by a culture different from the subject's own. Cross-cultural analysis regards cultural communication differences, on example what is the different conception of death in Italian culture than Korean (useful to understand how different rituals and behaviors are adopted, that otherwise would not find explanation), while inter-cultural analysis regard what happens during interaction, on example applying empathy at cultural level to know how to behave in that situation in a different country. Empathy interculturally regards a variety of issues, such as what is the cultural approach to time perception (deadlines, temporal precision, perspective time) in a North-European or Latin American culture (and how to act in cases of inter-cultural communication, keeping high effectiveness even within a different culture), how to negotiate with people from different cultures and organizations, and be able to integrate all possible difference of communication styles due to differences in culture. The literature distinguishes four levels of empathy, identified by the Italian researcher Daniele Trevisani (2005) that examines the dimensions useful for applying empathic component on the intercultural setting:
- Behavioral Empathy: understanding the behavior of a different culture and their causes, the ability to understand why the behavior is adopted and the chains of related behaviors.
- Emotional empathy: being able to feel the emotions experienced by others, even in cultures different from their own, understand what emotions feels the culturally different person (which emotion is flowing), of which intensity, which are the emotional lives, how emotions are associated to people, objects, events, situations, in private or public aspects of different cultures.
- Relational empathy: understanding the map of the relations of the subject and its affective value in the culture of belonging, to understand with whom the subject relates whether voluntarily or compulsorily, who has to deal with that subject in order to decide, in work or life, what is his map of "significant others ", the referents, the interlocutors, "other relevant "and influencers affecting their decisions, who are enemies and friends, who can affects his/her professional and life decisions.
- Cognitive empathy (understanding of different cognitive or prototypes): understanding the cognitive prototypes active in a given moment of time in a certain culture in a single person, the beliefs that generate the visible values, ideologies underlying behaviors, identifying the mental structures that the individuals own and which parts are culturally-depending" (Trevisani, 2005).[112]
Fiction
Some philosophers (such as Martha Nussbaum) suggest that novel reading cultivates readers' empathy and leads them to exercise better world citizenship. For a critique of this application of the empathy-altruism hypothesis to experiences of narrative empathy, see Keen's Empathy and the Novel (Oxford, 2007). In some works of science fiction and fantasy, empathy is understood to be a paranormal or psychic ability to sense the emotions of others, as opposed to telepathy, which allows one to perceive thoughts as well. A person who has that ability is also called an "empath" or "telepath" in this context. Occasionally these empaths are also able to project their own emotions, or to affect the emotions of others.[citation needed]
Measurement
This section has an unclear citation style. (July 2013) |
Research into the measurement of empathy has sought to answer a number of questions: who should be carrying out the measurement? What should pass for empathy and what should be discounted? What unit of measure (UOM) should be adopted and to what degree should each occurrence precisely match that UOM are also key questions that researchers have sought to investigate.
Researchers have approached the measurement of empathy from a number of perspectives.
Behavioural measures normally involve raters assessing the presence or absence of certain either predetermined or ad-hoc behaviours in the subjects they are monitoring. Both verbal and non-verbal behaviours have been captured on video by experimenters such as Truax (1967b).[117] Other experimenters, including Mehrabian and Epstein (1972),[118] have required subjects to comment upon their own feelings and behaviours, or those of other people involved in the experiment, as indirect ways of signalling their level of empathic functioning to the raters.
Physiological responses tend to be captured by elaborate electronic equipment that has been physically connected to the subject's body. Researchers then draw inferences about that person's empathic reactions from the electronic readings produced (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992;[119] Leslie et al., 2004[120]).
Bodily or "somatic" measures can be looked upon as behavioural measures at a micro level. Their focus is upon measuring empathy through facial and other non-verbally expressed reactions in the empathiser. These changes are presumably underpinned by physiological changes brought about by some form of "emotional contagion" or mirroring (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992*; Leslie et al., 2004*). It should be pointed out that these reactions, whilst appearing to reflect the internal emotional state of the empathiser, could also, if the stimulus incident lasted more than the briefest period, be reflecting the results of emotional reactions that are based upon more pieces of thinking through (cognitions) associated with role-taking ("if I were him I would feel ...").
Paper-based indices involve one or more of a variety of methods of responding. In some experiments, subjects are required to watch video scenarios (either staged or authentic) and to make written responses which are then assessed for their levels of empathy (e.g. Geher, Warner and Brown, 2001[121]); scenarios are sometimes also depicted in printed form (e.g. Mehrabian and Epstein, 1972[118]). Measures also frequently require subjects to self-report upon their own ability or capacity for empathy, using Likert-style numerical responses to a printed questionnaire that may have been designed to tap into the affective, cognitive-affective or largely cognitive substrates of empathic functioning. Some questionnaires claim to have been able to tap into both cognitive and affective substrates (e.g. Davis, 1980[122]). More recent paper-based tools include The Empathy Quotient (EQ) created by Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright[123] which comprises a self-report questionnaire consisting of 60 items.
For the very young, picture or puppet-story indices for empathy have been adopted to enable even very young, pre-school subjects to respond without needing to read questions and write answers (e.g. Denham and Couchoud, 1990). Dependent variables (variables that are monitored for any change by the experimenter) for younger subjects have included self reporting on a 7-point smiley face scale and filmed facial reactions (Barnett, 1984).[124]
A certain amount of confusion exists about how to measure empathy. These may be rooted in another problem: deciding what is empathy and what is not. In general, researchers have until now been keen to pin down a singular definition of empathy which would allow them to design a measure to assess its presence in an exchange, in someone's repertoire of behaviours or within them as a latent trait. As a result they have been frequently forced to ignore the richness of the empathic process in favour of capturing surface, explicit self-report or third-party data about whether empathy between two people was present or not. In most cases, instruments have unfortunately only yielded information on whether someone had the potential to demonstrate empathy (Geher et al., 2001)*. Gladstein (1987)[125] summarises the position noting that empathy has been measured from the point of view of the empathiser, the recipient for empathy and the third-party observer. He suggests that since the multiple measures used have produced results that bear little relation to one another, researchers should refrain from making comparisons between scales that are in fact measuring different things. He suggests that researchers should instead stipulate what kind of empathy they are setting out to measure rather than simplistically stating that they are setting out to measure the unitary phenomenon "empathy"; a view more recently endorsed by Duan and Hill (1996).[126]
In the field of medicine, a measurement tool for carers is the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, Health Professional Version (JSPE-HP).[127] At least one study using this tool with health sciences' students has found that levels of empathy are greater amongst females than males, and also are greater amongst older students than younger students.[128]
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is the only published measurement tool accounting for a multi-dimensional assessment of empathy, consisting of a self-report questionnaire of 28 items, divided into four 7-item scales covering the subdivisions of affective and cognitive empathy.[14][23]
Gender differences
The issue of gender differences in empathy is quite controversial. It is often believed that females are more empathic than males. Evidence for gender differences in empathy are important for self-report questionnaires of empathy in which it is obvious what was being indexed (e.g., impact of social desirability and gender stereotypes) but are smaller or nonexistent for other types of indexes that are less self-evident with regard to their purpose.[129] On average female subjects score higher than males on the Empathy Quotient (EQ), while males tend to score higher on the Systemizing Quotient (SQ).
Both males and females with Autistic Spectrum Disorders usually score higher on the SQ (Baron-Cohen, 2003).[17] However, a series of recent studies, using a variety of neurophysiological measures, including MEG,[130] spinal reflex excitability,[131] and electroencephalography[132][133] have documented the presence of a gender difference in the human mirror neuron system, with female participants exhibiting stronger motor resonance than male participants. In addition, these aforementioned studies found that female participants scored higher on empathy self-report dispositional measures and that these measures positively correlated with the physiological response. However, other studies show that women do not possess greater empathic abilities than men, and perceived gender differences are the result of motivational differences.[134][135] Using fMRI, neuroscientist Tania Singer showed that empathy-related neural responses are significantly lower in males when observing an "unfair" person experiencing pain.[136]
See also
References
- ^ "Empathy". plato.stanford.edu. 2008-03-31. Retrieved 2012-08-29.
- ^ Empatheia, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, at Perseus
- ^ Gallese, Vittorio (2003). "The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity". Psychopathology. 36 (4): 171–180. doi:10.1159/000072786. PMID 14504450. CiteSeerx: 10.1.1.143.2396.
- ^ Bar-On, Reuven; Parker, James DA (2000). The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace. San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 0-7879-4984-1. pp. 40–59
- ^ Taylor (1997), pp. 28–31
- ^ Hodges, S.D., & Klein, K.J. (2001). Regulating the costs of empathy: the price of being human. Journal of Socio-Economics.
- ^ Happiness Genes: Unlock the Positive Potential Hidden in Your DNA, New Page Books (April, 2010) ISBN 978-1-60163-105-3
- ^ O'Malley W. J. (1999). "Teaching Empathy". America. 180 (12): 22–26.
- ^ Schwartz W (2002). "From passivity to competence: A conceptualization of knowledge, skill, tolerance, and empathy". Psychiatry. 65 (4): 338–345.
- ^ Schwartz W (In press). "The parameters of empathy". Advances in Descriptive Psychology. 10.
{{cite journal}}
: Check date values in:|year=
(help)CS1 maint: year (link) - ^ Meltzoff A.N., Decety J. (2003). "What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience". The Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London. 358 (1431): 491–500. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1261.
- ^ Batson, C.D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In J. Decety and W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: MIT Press
- ^ Hatfield E., Cacioppo J. L., Rapson R. L. (1993). "Emotional contagion" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Sciences. 2 (3): 96–99. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770953.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (2007). "Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 37 (4): 709–15. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Judith Aharon-Peretz and Daniella Perry (2009). "Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions". Brain. 132 (3): 617–627. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279.
- ^ Frans B.M. deWaal (2008). "Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy" (PDF). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 (59): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PMID 17550343.
- ^ a b c The Essential Difference: The Truth about the Male and Female Brain, Basic Books (July 1, 2003) ISBN 978-0-7382-0844-2
- ^ Kimberley Rogers, Isabel Dziobek, Jason Hassenstab, Oliver T. Wolf, Antonio Convit (2007). "Who Cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 37 (4): 709–715. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. (2012). "The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics". Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 7 (6): 727–37. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr051. PMC 3427869. PMID 21896497.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f g h i Ilaria Minio-Paluello, Michael V. Lombardo, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Sally Wheelwright, Simon Baron-Cohen (2009). "Response to Smith's Letter to the Editor 'Emotional Empathy in Autism Spectrum Conditions: Weak, Intact, or Heightened?'" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 39 (12): 1749. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0800-x.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c Baron-Cohen, Simon (2011). Zero Degrees of Empathy: A New Theory of Human Cruelty. Penguin UK. Retrieved August 8, 2013.
- ^ Yan Fan, Niall W. Duncana, Moritz de Greck, Georg Northoff (2011). "Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 35 (3): 903–911. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d Davis, M. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126.
- ^ a b Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural basis of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- ^ Decety J., Meyer M. (2008). "From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account". Development and Psychopathology. 20 (4): 1053–1080. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000503. PMID 18838031.
- ^ Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006) Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (pp. 517–549). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- ^ Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people's action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9
- ^ Zahn-Waxler C., Radke-Yarrow M. (1990). "The origins of empathic concern". Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 107–130. doi:10.1007/BF00991639.
- ^ Decety J., Michalska K.J., Akitsuki Y. (2008). "Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children". Neuropsychologia. 46 (11): 2607–2614. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.026. PMID 18573266.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Brain Scans Show Children Naturally Prone to Empathy Newswise, retrieved on July 13, 2008.
- ^ Wimmer H., Perner J. (1983). "Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception". Cognition. 13 (1): 103–28. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. PMID 6681741.
- ^ Olsen, Douglas (September 2001). "Empathetic Maturity: Theory of Moral Point of View in Clinical Relations". Advances in Nursing Science. 24 (1): 36–46. PMID 11554532.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: date and year (link) - ^ Keysers, C. & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions and sensations. Curr Opinion Neurobiol, 2009
- ^ Decety J., Moriguchi Y. (2007). "The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions". BioPsychoSocial Medicine. 1: 22–65. doi:10.1186/1751-0759-1-22.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Wicker B.; Keysers, Christian; Plailly, Jane; Royet, Jean-Pierre; Gallese, Vittorio; Rizzolatti, Giacomo; et al. (2003). "Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Keysers, C. et al. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch, Neuron, 42:335-46
- ^ Blakemore S.-J.; Bristow, D; Bird, G; Frith, C; Ward, J; et al. (2005). "Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia". Brain. 128 (Pt 7): 1571–1583. doi:10.1093/brain/awh500. PMID 15817510.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Morrison, I., Lloyd, D., di Pellegrino, G., & Roberts, N. (2004). Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue? Cognitive & Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 270-278.
- ^ Jackson P.L., Meltzoff A.N., Decety J. (2005). "How do we perceive the pain of others: A window into the neural processes involved in empathy". NeuroImage. 24 (3): 771–779. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006. PMID 15652312.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Singer T.; Seymour; O'Doherty; Kaube; Dolan; Frith; et al. (2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain". Science. 303 (5661): 1157–1161. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Preston S., de Waal F. (2002). "Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25: 1–72.
- ^ Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during obser- vation of outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (2010), doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011
- ^ Jack, Anthony I.; Dawson, Abigail J.; Begany, Katelyn L.; Leckie, Regina L.; Barry, Kevin P.; Ciccia, Angela H.; Snyder, Abraham Z. (2013). "FMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains". NeuroImage. 66: 385. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.061.
- ^ http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/10/121030161416.htm
- ^ a b Hoffman, Martin L. (1990). "Empathy and justice motivation". Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 151. doi:10.1007/BF00991641. Retrieved 2012-08-29.
- ^ Abstract: Assessing a new dimension of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and punishing desires, Vitaglione, Guy D. & Barnett, Mark A. [1]
- ^ Mohr P., Howells K., Gerace A., Day A., Wharton M. (2007). "The role of perspective taking in anger arousal". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (3): 507–517. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.019.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Day A., Mohr P., Howells K., Gerace A., Lim L. (2012). "The role of empathy in anger arousal in violent offenders and university students". International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 56 (4): 599–613. doi:10.1177/0306624X11431061.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c Decety, J., Michalska, K.J., Akitsuki, Y., & Lahey, B. (2008). "Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation". Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–11. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC 2819310. PMID 18940230.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Grossman, Dave (1996). On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society. Back Bay Books. ISBN 0-316-33000-0.
- ^ a b Hill E., Berthoz S., Frith U (2004). "'Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives.'" (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 229–235. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022613.41399.14. PMID 15162941.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Taylor, G.J. and Bagby, R.M & Parker, J.D.A. Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. (1997) Cambridge Uni. Press.
- ^ Sifneos PE. The prevalence of 'alexithymic' characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 22 (2):255-262, 1973
- ^ Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. Empathy and judging other's pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex (2007); Bird, J., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U., and Frith, C. Alexithymia In Autistic Spectrum Disorders: and fMRI Investigation (2006) : and Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U & C. Alexithymia in Autism Spectrum Disorders: an fMRI Investigation (2006).
- ^ Brackett; Warner, Rebecca M.; Bosco, Jennifer S.; et al. (2005). "'Emotional Intelligence and Relationship Quality Among Couples'" (PDF). Personal Relationships. 12 (2): 197–212. doi:10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00111.x.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Yelsma, P., Marrow, S. - 'An Examination of Couples' Difficulties With Emotional Expressiveness and Their Marital Satisfaction' in Journal of Family Communication 3 (2003) p. 41–62 [2]
- ^ Allen. - 'Autism, music, and the therapeutic potential of music in alexithymia' in Music Perception (2010) p.251
- ^ "Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism".
- ^ Dapretto M., Davies M.S., Pfeifer J.H., Scott A.A., Sigman M., Bookheimer S.Y., Iacoboni M. (2006). "Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (1): 28–31. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMID 16327784.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Oberman. - 'EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum' in Brain Research (2005) p.190
- ^ IMFAR 2007 abstract http://www.autism-insar.org/docs/IMFAR2007_Program.pdf
- ^ Gillberg, C. L. (1992). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1991. Autism and autistic-like conditions: subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 813–842.
- ^ Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., Ponnet, K., & Pichal, B. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 271–278.
- ^ Phoebe Caldwell, "Letters", London Times, Dec 30 2005
- ^ a b c Simon Baron-Cohen (2009). "Autism: The Empathizing–Systemizing (E-S) Theory". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156 (The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009). New York Academy of Sciences: 68–80. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156...68B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x. Retrieved July 29, 2013.
- ^ Baron-Cohen. 'Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism' in Science (2005) p.819
- ^ Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Br. J. Psychol., 100, 1–22.
- ^ "Autism 'affects male and female brains differently'". BBC News. August 9, 2013. Retrieved August 9, 2013.
- ^ Minzenberg, M.J. (2006). "Reduced self-referential source memory performance is associated with interpersonal dysfunction in borderline personality disorder" (PDF). Journal of Personality Disorders.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b c d Harari, Hagai (March 1, 2009). "Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder". Psychiatry Research.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Wagner, A.W. (1999). "Facial expression recognition ability among women with borderline personality disorder: implications for emotion regulation?". Journal of Personality Disorders.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Lynch, T.R. (2006). "Heightened sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline personality disorder". Emotion.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Cleckly, H. C. (1941). "The Mask of Sanity: An attempt to Reinterpret the So-Called Psychopathic Personality". St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Hare, R. D. (1991). "The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised". Toronto: Multi Health Systems.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ a b Blair, R.J. (2001a). "Somatic markers and response reversal: is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies?". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Blair, R. J. (2002). "Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals". Journal of Abnormal Psychology.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Stevens, D. (2001). "Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies". Journal of Genetic Psychology.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ Blair, R.J. (1995). "A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath" (PDF). Cognition.
- ^ Blair, R.J.R. (2003). "Neurobiological basis of psychopathy". British Journal of Psychiatry.
- ^ a b "Psychopathy" by Quinton 2006
- ^ Hogenboom, Melissa (July 25, 2013). "Psychopathic criminals have empathy switch". BBC News. Retrieved July 28, 2013.
- ^ Narcissistic personality disorder – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
- ^ Schizoid personality disorder, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
- ^ http://www.mentalhealth.com/icd/p22-pe02.html Schizoid personality disorder - International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10)]
- ^ Guntrip, Harry. Schizoid Phenomena, Object-Relations, and The Self. New York: International Universities Press, 1969.
- ^ Ralph Klein- pp. 13–23 in Disorders of the Self: New Therapeutic Horizons, Brunner/Mazel (1995).
- ^ Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101(1), 91–119.
- ^ Bjorkqvist, K., Osterman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (2000). Social intelligence-empathy = aggression? Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5(2), 191–200.
- ^ Geer, J. H., Estupinan, L. A., & Manguno-Mire, G. M. (2000). Empathy, social skills, and other relevant cognitive processes in rapists and child molesters. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 5(1), 99–126.
- ^ a b de Waal F. B. M. (2008). "Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy". Annual Review of Psychology. 59: 279–300.
- ^ Wyschogrod E (1981). "Empathy and sympathy as tactile encounter". J Med Philos. 6 (1): 25–43. doi:10.1093/jmp/6.1.25. PMID 7229562.
- ^ http://www.wired.co.uk/news/archive/2012-07/12/tania-singer-compassion-burnout
- ^ How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time: Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, (2008), ISBN 978-1-84706-347-2
- ^ a b Iain King (16 October 2008). How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time. Continuum. p. 256. ISBN 978-1-84706-347-2. Retrieved 28 August 2013. Page 74 states "Empathy is special, because it always and automatically has the characteristics of right and wrong ... Something rooted in empathy must have more of the essence of good about it than something which is not". Cite error: The named reference "King2008" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
- ^ Peter Vardy and Charlotte Vardy (April 2012). Ethics Matters. SCM Press. p. 256. ISBN 978-0-334-04391-1. Retrieved 28 August 2013. Page 116 of this book states: In How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time: Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, London: Continuum 2008, Iain King develops a system compatible with consequence-, virtue- and act based ethics.
- ^ A Buddhist account of Iain King's ideas is set out in this Global Oneness article.
- ^ Publisher's Weekly state that "King is even able to formulate a credible rule that tells us when to lie" here.
- ^ The Ethics of Care and Empathy, Michael Slote, Oxford University Press, 2007
- ^ Empathy in the Context of Philosophy, Lou Agosta, Palgrave/Macmillan, 2010
- ^ Jenkins, K. (1991) Re-thinking History London: Routledge
- ^ Pozzi, G. (1976) Prefazione 6. L'elemento storico e politico -sociale, in G.B. Marino, L'Adone Milano
- ^ White, T. I. (2007). In defense of dolphins: the new moral frontier. Malden, MA: Blackwell Pub..
- ^ Sandin, Jo (2007). Bonobos: Encounters in Empathy. Milwaukee: Zoological Society of Milwaukee & The Foundation for Wildlife Conservation, Inc. p. 109. ISBN 978-0-9794151-0-4.
- ^ The age of empathy: nature's lessons for a kinder society By: Waal, F. B. M. de. Harmony Books 2009
- ^ Ben-Ami Bartal I., Decety J., Mason P.; Decety; Mason (2011). "Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats". Science. 334 (6061): 1427–1430. Bibcode:2011Sci...334.1427B. doi:10.1126/science.1210789. PMID 22158823.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Dale J. Langford, Sara E. Crager, Zarrar Shehzad, Shad B. Smith, Susana G. Sotocinal, Jeremy S. Levenstadt, Mona Lisa Chanda, Daniel J. Levitin, Jeffrey S. Mogil (June 30, 2006). "Social Modulation of Pain as Evidence for Empathy in Mice". Science. 312 (5782): 1967–1970. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1967L. doi:10.1126/science.1128322. PMID 16809545.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Decety J (2011). "The neuroevolution of empathy". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1231: 35–45. Bibcode:2011NYASA1231...35D. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x. PMID 21651564.
- ^ Decety J., Svetlova M. (2012). "Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy". Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2 (1): 1–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003. PMID 22682726.
- ^ Aronson, Elliot (2007). Social Psychology, 6th Edition. Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-238245-8.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|coauthors=
ignored (|author=
suggested) (help) - ^ a b Trevisani, Daniele (2005) Negoziazione Interculturale. Comunicazione oltre le barriere culturali, Milan: Franco Angeli (Title translation: Intercultural Negotiation: Communication Beyond Cultural Barriers)
- ^ Cornelius-White,J. H. D., and A. P. Harbaugh. (2010). Learner-Centered Instruction.Thousand Oaks, CA,London, New Delhi, Singapore: SAGE Publications.
- ^ http://www.wiredtocare.com
- ^ Miyashiro, Marie R. (2011). The Empathy Factor: Your Competitive Advantage for Personal, Team, and Business Success. Puddledancer Press. p. 256. ISBN 1-892005-25-5.
- ^ Dowden, Craig (June 21, 2013). "Forget ethics training: Focus on empathy". The National Post. Archived from the original on July 24, 2013.
- ^ Truax, C. B. (1967). Rating of Accurate Empathy. The Therapeutic Relationship and its Impact. A Study of Psychotherapy with Schizophrenics. Eds. C. R. Rogers, E. T. Gendlin, D. J. Kiesler and C. B. Truax. Madison, Wisconsin, The University of Wisconsin Press pp. 555–568.
- ^ a b Mehrabian A., Epstein N. (1972). "A measure of emotional empathy". Journal of Personality. 40 (4): 525–543. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00078.x. PMID 4642390.
- ^ Levenson R. W., Ruef A. M. (1992). "Empathy: a physiological substrate". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63 (2): 234–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234. PMID 1403614.
- ^ Leslie K. R., Johnson-Frey S. H.; et al. (2004). "Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy". NeuroImage. 21 (2): 601–607. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.038. PMID 14980562.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Geher G., Warner R. M.; et al. (2001). "Predictive validity of the emotional accuracy research scale". Intelligence. 29 (5): 373–388. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00045-3.
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Davis M. H. (1980). "A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy". JSAS Catalogue of selected documents in psychology. 10 (4): 1–17.
- ^ Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. (2004). "The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 163–175. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00. PMID 15162935.
- ^ Barnett M. A. (1984). "Similarity of experience and empathy in preschoolers". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 145 (2): 241–250. doi:10.1080/00221325.1984.10532271.
- ^ Gladstein, G. A. (1987). What It All Means. Empathy and counseling: explorations in theory and research. G. A. Gladstein. New York, Springer-Verlag: 173-189.
- ^ Duan C., Hill C. E. (1996). "The current state of empathy research". Journal of Counselling Psychology. 43 (3): 261–274. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.43.3.261.
- ^ Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J (2007). "A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy". J Gen Intern Med. 22 (10): 1434–8. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0298-x. PMC 2305857. PMID 17653807.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Boyle M; et al. (2010). "Levels of empathy in undergraduate health science students". The Internet Journal of Medical Education. 1 (1).
{{cite journal}}
: Explicit use of et al. in:|author=
(help) - ^ Eisenberg N., Fabes R.A. (1990). "Empathy: Conceptualization, Measurement, and Relation to Prosocial Behavior". Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 131–149. doi:10.1007/BF00991640.
- ^ Cheng Y., Tzeng O.J., Decety J., Hsieh J.C. (2006). "Gender differences in the human mirror system: a magnetoencephalography study". NeuroReport. 17 (11): 1115–1119. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21. PMID 16837838.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Cheng Y., Decety J., Hsieh J.C., Hung D., Tzeng O.J. (2007). "Gender differences in spinal excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion". NeuroReport. 18 (9): 887–890. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280ebb486. PMID 17515795.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Yang C.Y., Decety J., Lee S., Chen G., Cheng Y. (2009). "Gender differences in the Mu rhythm during empathy for pain: An electroencephalographic study". Brain Research. 1251: 176–184. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.062. PMID 19083993.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Cheng Y., Lee P., Yang C.Y., Lin C.P., Decety J.; Lee; Yang; Lin; Hung; Decety (2008). Rustichini, Aldo (ed.). "Gender differences in the mu rhythm of the human mirror-neuron system". PLoS ONE. 5 (5): e2113. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2113C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002113.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Ickes, W. (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press.
- ^ Klein K. Hodges S. (2001). "Gender Differences, Motivation, and Empathic Accuracy: When it Pays to Understand". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27 (6): 720–730. doi:10.1177/0146167201276007.
- ^ Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ, Frith CD; Seymour; O'Doherty; Stephan; Dolan; Frith (2006). "Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others". Nature. 439 (7075): 466–9. Bibcode:2006Natur.439..466S. doi:10.1038/nature04271. PMC 2636868. PMID 16421576.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|month=
ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
External links
- The dictionary definition of empathy at Wiktionary
- Entry on empathy at Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy
- Rogers, CR, Lyon, HC Jr, Tausch, R: (2013) On Becoming an Effective Teacher - Person-centered teaching,psychology, philosophy, anddialogues with Carl R. Rogers and Harold Lyon London: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-81698-4: http://www.routledge.com/9780415816984/