Jump to content

Affective neuroscience: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
m v2.05b - Bot T20 CW#61 - Fix errors for CW project (Reference before punctuation)
Mfs2162 (talk | contribs)
simplified language for intro to brain structures, limbic structures, and other brain structures
Line 2: Line 2:
{{Psychology sidebar}}
{{Psychology sidebar}}


Emotions are thought to be related to activity in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and help us make decisions about our environment. Early stages of research on emotions and the brain was conducted by [[Paul Broca]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Broca |first=Paul |date=1878 |title=Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales: le grand lobe limbique |journal=Rev. Anthropology |volume=1 |pages=385-498}}</ref>, [[James Papez]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=PAPEZ |first=JAMES W. |date=1937-10-01 |title=A PROPOSED MECHANISM OF EMOTION |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003 |journal=Archives of Neurology And Psychiatry |volume=38 |issue=4 |pages=725 |doi=10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003 |issn=0096-6754}}</ref>, and [[Paul D. MacLean]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=MacLean |first=Paul D. |date=1952 |title=Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (Visceral brain) |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4 |journal=Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology |volume=4 |issue=4 |pages=407–418 |doi=10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4 |issn=0013-4694}}</ref>. Their work suggests that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the [[limbic system]]. The limbic system is made up of the following brain structures:
'''Affective neuroscience''' is the study of the how the [[brain]] processes [[Emotion|emotions]]. This field combines [[neuroscience]] with the [[Psychology|psychological study]] of [[Personality psychology|personality]], [[emotion]], and [[Mood (psychology)|mood]].<ref>{{Citation |last=Panksepp |first=Jaak |title=A Role for Affective Neuroscience in Understanding Stress: The Case of Separation Distress Circuitry |date=1990 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-1990-7_4 |work=Psychobiology of Stress |pages=41–57 |place=Dordrecht |publisher=Springer Netherlands |isbn=978-94-010-7390-5 |access-date=2022-11-27}}</ref> The basis of emotions and what emotions are remains an issue of debate within the field of affective neuroscience.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Celeghin |first=Alessia |last2=Diano |first2=Matteo |last3=Bagnis |first3=Arianna |last4=Viola |first4=Marco |last5=Tamietto |first5=Marco |date=2017-08-24 |title=Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432 |journal=Frontiers in Psychology |volume=8 |doi=10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432 |issn=1664-1078}}</ref>
===Limbic system===


* [[Amygdala]] – The amygdala is made up of two small, round structures located closer to the forehead ([[anterior]]) to the [[Hippocampus|hippocampi]] near the [[Temporal lobe|temporal]] poles. The amygdalae are involved in detecting and learning which parts of our surroundings are important and have emotional significance. They are critical for the production of emotion. They are known to be very important for negative emotions, especially fear<ref>{{Cite journal |last=LeDoux |first=Joseph E. |date=1995 |title=Emotion: Clues from the Brain |url=https://www.annualreviews.org/doi/10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233 |journal=Annual Review of Psychology |language=en |volume=46 |issue=1 |pages=209–235 |doi=10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233 |issn=0066-4308}}</ref>. Amygdala activation often happens when we see a potential threat. The amygdala uses our past, related memories to help us make decisions about what is currently happening<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Breiter |first=Hans C |last2=Etcoff |first2=Nancy L |last3=Whalen |first3=Paul J |last4=Kennedy |first4=William A |last5=Rauch |first5=Scott L |last6=Buckner |first6=Randy L |last7=Strauss |first7=Monica M |last8=Hyman |first8=Steven E |last9=Rosen |first9=Bruce R |date=1996 |title=Response and Habituation of the Human Amygdala during Visual Processing of Facial Expression |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0896627300802196 |journal=Neuron |language=en |volume=17 |issue=5 |pages=875–887 |doi=10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6}}</ref>.
The term "affective neuroscience" was coined by neuroscientist [[Jaak Panksepp]], at a time when [[cognitive neuroscience]]<nowiki/>focused on parts of psychology that did not include emotion, such as attention or memory<ref>{{Cite book |last=Panksepp |first=Jaak |title=Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions |publisher=Oxford University Press |year=2004 |isbn=978-0195178050 |edition=1st |language=English}}</ref>.{{toclimit|3}}
* [[Thalamus]] – The thalamus is involved in combining sensory and motor signals and then sending that information to the [[cerebral cortex]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sherman |first=S. |date=2006 |title=Thalamus |url=http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Thalamus |journal=Scholarpedia |language=en |volume=1 |issue=9 |pages=1583 |doi=10.4249/scholarpedia.1583 |issn=1941-6016}}</ref>. The thalamus plays an important role in regulating [[Neuroscience of sleep|sleep and wakefulness]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Steriade |first=M |last2=Llinás |first2=R R |date=1988 |title=The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. |url=https://www.physiology.org/doi/10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649 |journal=Physiological Reviews |language=en |volume=68 |issue=3 |pages=649–742 |doi=10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649 |issn=0031-9333}}</ref>.
==Brain areas==
* [[Hypothalamus]] – The hypothalamus is involved in producing a physical response (for example, crying) with an emotion. The hypothalamus is also used in [[Reward system|reward circuits]] which are associated with [[Broaden-and-build|positive emotions]]<ref>{{Cite book |url=https://www.worldcat.org/oclc/825767970 |title=The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience |date=2013 |publisher=Cambridge University Press |others=Jorge Armony, Patrik Vuilleumier |isbn=978-1-107-31401-6 |location=Cambridge |oclc=825767970}}</ref>.
Emotions are thought to be related to activity in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and choose the significance of what is going on around us. Pioneering work by [[Paul Broca]] (1878),<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Broca | first1 = P. | year = 1878 | title = Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales: le grand lobe limbique | journal = Rev. Anthropol | volume = 1 | pages = 385–498 }}</ref> [[James Papez]] (1937),<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Papez | first1 = J.W. | year = 1937 | title = A proposed mechanism of emotion | journal = J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci | volume = 7 | issue = 1| pages = 103–12 | pmid = 7711480 | doi = 10.1176/jnp.7.1.103 }}</ref> and [[Paul D. MacLean]] (1952)<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Maclean | first1 = P.D. | year = 1952 | title = Psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain) | journal = Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology| volume = 4 | issue = 4| pages = 407–18 | doi=10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4 | pmid=12998590}}</ref> suggested that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the [[limbic system]], which includes the [[hypothalamus]], [[cingulate cortex]], [[hippocampus|hippocampi]], and other structures. Research has shown that limbic structures are directly related to emotion, but other structures have been found to be of greater emotional relevance. The following brain structures are currently thought to be involved in emotion:<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Dalgleish | first1 = T. | year = 2004 | title = The emotional brain | journal = Nature Reviews Neuroscience | volume = 5 | issue = 7| pages = 583–9 | doi=10.1038/nrn1432| pmid = 15208700 | s2cid = 148864726 }}</ref>
* [[Hippocampus]] – The hippocampus is a structure that is mainly involved in [[memory]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rolls |first=Edmund T. |date=2022 |title=The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0301008222001204 |journal=Progress in Neurobiology |language=en |volume=217 |pages=102334 |doi=10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334}}</ref>. It works to make new memories and also connects senses such as visual input, smell or sound to memories. The hippocampus allows [[Long-term memory|long term memories]] to be stored and retrieves them when necessary<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Fischer |first=Håkan |last2=Wright |first2=Christopher I |last3=Whalen |first3=Paul J |last4=McInerney |first4=Sean C |last5=Shin |first5=Lisa M |last6=Rauch |first6=Scott L |date=2003 |title=Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: A functional MRI study |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0361923002009401 |journal=Brain Research Bulletin |language=en |volume=59 |issue=5 |pages=387–392 |doi=10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00940-1}}</ref>.
* [[Fornix of the brain|Fornix]] – The fornix is the main connection between the hippocampus to the mammillary bodies. It is important for [[spatial memory]] functions, [[episodic memory]] and [[executive functions]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Modi |first=Shilpi |last2=Trivedi |first2=Richa |last3=Singh |first3=Kavita |last4=Kumar |first4=Pawan |last5=Rathore |first5=Ram K.S. |last6=Tripathi |first6=Rajendra P. |last7=Khushu |first7=Subash |date=2013 |title=Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fasciculus: Preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0166432812006468 |journal=Behavioural Brain Research |language=en |volume=238 |pages=188–192 |doi=10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.007}}</ref>.
* [[Mammillary body]] – Mammillary bodies are important for [[Episodic memory|recollective memory]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Vann |first=Seralynne D. |date=2010 |title=Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0028393209004229 |journal=Neuropsychologia |language=en |volume=48 |issue=8 |pages=2316–2327 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.019}}</ref>. They are found by the [[Brainstem|brain stem]] and [[cerebrum]].
* [[Olfactory bulb]] – The olfactory bulbs are the first [[cranial nerves]] <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Imai |first=Takeshi |date=2014 |title=Construction of functional neuronal circuitry in the olfactory bulb |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.07.012 |journal=Seminars in Cell &amp; Developmental Biology |volume=35 |pages=180–188 |doi=10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.07.012 |issn=1084-9521}}</ref>. They are involved in smell ([[olfaction]]) and memory that is connected with specific smells <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Tong |first=Michelle T. |last2=Peace |first2=Shane T. |last3=Cleland |first3=Thomas A. |date=2014-07-07 |title=Properties and mechanisms of olfactory learning and memory |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00238 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=8 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00238 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref>.
* [[Cingulate gyrus]] – The cingulate gyrus is located above the [[corpus callosum]] <ref>{{Citation |last=Rolls |first=Edmund T. |title=The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory |date=2019 |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/B9780444641960000029 |work=Handbook of Clinical Neurology |volume=166 |pages=23–37 |publisher=Elsevier |language=en |doi=10.1016/b978-0-444-64196-0.00002-9 |isbn=978-0-444-64196-0 |access-date=2022-11-27}}</ref>. The parts of the cingulate gyrus have different functions, and are involved with affect, visceromotor control, response selection, skeletomotor control, visuospatial processing, and in memory access<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Weissman |first=D.H. |date=2004-07-06 |title=Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Resolves Conflict from Distracting Stimuli by Boosting Attention toward Relevant Events |url=https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/bhh125 |journal=Cerebral Cortex |language=en |volume=15 |issue=2 |pages=229–237 |doi=10.1093/cercor/bhh125 |issn=1460-2199}}</ref>. The [[anterior cingulate cortex]] is important for conscious, subjective emotional awareness as well as [[motivation]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Medford |first=Nick |last2=Critchley |first2=Hugo D. |date=2010 |title=Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response |url=http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x |journal=Brain Structure and Function |language=en |volume=214 |issue=5-6 |pages=535–549 |doi=10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x |issn=1863-2653 |pmc=PMC2886906 |pmid=20512367}}</ref>. The [[subgenual cingulate]] is more active during both experimentally induced [[sadness]] and during [[Major depressive episode|depressive episodes]]<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Drevets |first=Wayne C. |last2=Savitz |first2=Jonathan |last3=Trimble |first3=Michael |date=2008 |title=The Subgenual Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Mood Disorders |url=https://www.cambridge.org/core/product/identifier/S1092852900013754/type/journal_article |journal=CNS Spectrums |language=en |volume=13 |issue=8 |pages=663–681 |doi=10.1017/S1092852900013754 |issn=1092-8529 |pmc=PMC2729429 |pmid=18704022}}</ref>.


Research has shown the limbic system is directly related to emotion, but there are other brain areas and structures that are important for producing and processing emotion<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Dalgleish |first=Tim |date=2004 |title=The emotional brain |url=http://www.nature.com/articles/nrn1432 |journal=Nature Reviews Neuroscience |language=en |volume=5 |issue=7 |pages=583–589 |doi=10.1038/nrn1432 |issn=1471-003X}}</ref>.
===Limbic system===
* [[Amygdala]] – The amygdalae are two small, round structures located [[anterior]] to the [[hippocampus|hippocampi]] near the [[temporal lobe|temporal]] poles. The amygdalae are involved in detecting and learning which parts of our surroundings are important and have emotional significance. They are critical for the production of emotion, and may be particularly so for negative emotions, especially fear.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Ledoux | first1 = J.E | year = 1995 | title = Emotion: clues from the brain | journal = Annual Review of Psychology | volume = 46 | pages = 209–35 | doi=10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233| pmid = 7872730 }}</ref> Multiple studies have shown amygdala activation when perceiving a potential threat; various circuits allow the amygdala to use related past memories to better judge the possible threat.<ref name="1996 Breiter">{{cite journal|last=Breiter|first=Hans|author2=N. Etcoff |title=Response and Habituation of the Human Amygdala during Visual Processing of Facial Expression|journal=Neuron|date=November 1996|volume=17|issue=5|pages=875–887|doi=10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80219-6|pmid=8938120|last3=Whalen|first3=Paul J|last4=Kennedy|first4=William A|last5=Rauch|first5=Scott L|last6=Buckner|first6=Randy L|last7=Strauss|first7=Monica M|last8=Hyman|first8=Steven E|last9=Rosen|first9=Bruce R|s2cid=17284478|doi-access=free}}</ref>
*[[Thalamus]] – The thalamus is involved in relaying sensory and motor signals to the cerebral cortex,<ref name="Poetics">{{cite journal |doi=10.4249/scholarpedia.1583 |title=Thalamus |year=2006 |last1=Sherman |first1=S. |journal=Scholarpedia |volume=1 |issue=9 |pages=1583|bibcode=2006SchpJ...1.1583S |doi-access=free }}</ref> especially visual stimuli. The thalamus plays an important role in regulating states of [[neuroscience of sleep|sleep and wakefulness]].<ref>{{cite journal |pmid=2839857 |year=1988 |last1=Steriade |first1=Mircea |last2=Llinás |first2=Rodolfo R. |title=The Functional States of the Thalamus and the Associated Neuronal Interplay |volume=68 |issue=3 |pages=649–742 |journal=Physiological Reviews |doi=10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649 }}</ref>
*[[Hypothalamus]] – The hypothalamus is involved in producing a physical output associated with an emotion as well as in reward circuits.<ref>{{cite book|editor-first=Jorge |editor-last=Armony|title=The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience|date=2013|publisher=Cambridge Univ. Press|location=Cambridge [u.a.]|isbn=978-0-521-17155-7|pages=421, 29 and 616|edition=1. publ.}}</ref>
*[[Hippocampus]] – The hippocampus is a structure of the medial temporal lobes that is mainly involved in memory.<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rolls |first=Edmund T. |date=October 2022 |title=The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334 |journal=Progress in Neurobiology |volume=217 |pages=102334 |doi=10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334 |pmid=35870682 |s2cid=250722212 |issn=0301-0082}}</ref> It works to form new memories and also connects senses such as visual input, smell or sound to memories. The hippocampus allows long term memories to be stored and retrieves them when necessary. Memories are used within the amygdala to help evaluate stimuli.<ref name="2001 Fischer">{{cite journal|last=Fischer|first=Hakan|author2=C.Wright |title=Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: a functional MRI study|journal=Brain Research Bulletin|year=2002|volume=59|issue=5|pages=387–92|doi=10.1016/s0361-9230(02)00940-1 |pmid=12507690|last3=Whalen|first3=Paul J|last4=McInerney|first4=Sean C|last5=Shin|first5=Lisa M|last6=Rauch|first6=Scott L|s2cid=43705711}}</ref>
*[[Fornix of the brain|Fornix]] – The fornix is the main output pathway from the hippocampus to the mammillary bodies. It has been identified as a main region in controlling spatial memory functions, episodic memory and executive functions.<ref name="2012 Modi">{{cite journal|last=Modi|first=Shilpi|author2=R. Trivedi|author3=K. Singh|author4=P. Kumar|author5=R. Rathore|author6=R. Tripathi|author7= S. Khushu|title=Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fascicles: Preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study|journal=Behavioural Brain Research|year=2012|volume=238|pages=188–192|doi=10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.007|pmid=23085341|s2cid=1741104}}</ref>
*[[Mammillary body]] – Mammillary bodies are important for [[episodic memory|recollective memory]].<ref>{{cite journal | url=http://psych.cf.ac.uk/home2/vann/VannNeuro2010.pdf | title=Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory | author=Seralynne D. Vann | journal=Neuropsychologia | year=2010 | volume=48 | issue=8 | pages=2316–2327 | doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.019| pmid=19879886 | s2cid=2424758 }}</ref>
*[[Olfactory bulb]] – The olfactory bulbs are the first [[cranial nerves]], located on the ventral side of the frontal lobe. They are involved in [[olfaction]], the perception of [[odors]].<ref name="urlolfactory bulb">{{cite web|url=http://www.sci.uidaho.edu/med532/olfacbulb.htm |title=olfactory bulb |access-date=2009-11-20 |url-status=dead |archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20091116125033/http://www.sci.uidaho.edu/med532/olfacbulb.htm |archive-date=2009-11-16 }}</ref>
*[[Cingulate gyrus]] – The cingulate gyrus is located above the [[corpus callosum]] and is usually considered to be part of the limbic system.<ref>{{Citation |last=Rolls |first=Edmund T. |title=The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory |date=2019 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-444-64196-0.00002-9 |work=Cingulate Cortex |series=Handbook of Clinical Neurology |volume=166 |pages=23–37 |publisher=Elsevier |doi=10.1016/b978-0-444-64196-0.00002-9 |pmid=31731913 |isbn=9780444641960 |s2cid=208060755 |access-date=2022-10-27}}</ref> The parts of the cingulate gyrus have different functions, and are involved with affect, visceromotor control, response selection, skeletomotor control, visuospatial processing, and in memory access.<ref>{{cite web | url=http://www.cingulumneurosciences.org/functional.shtml | title=Cingulate Gyrus: Functional Correlations of the 4 Cingulate Regions | publisher=Cingulum NeuroSciences Institute. | access-date=2013-04-23 | author1=Brent A. | author2=Leslie J. Vogt. | archive-url=https://web.archive.org/web/20130318233527/http://www.cingulumneurosciences.org/functional.shtml | archive-date=2013-03-18 | url-status=dead }}</ref> A part of the cingulate gyrus is the [[anterior cingulate cortex]], which is thought to play a central role in attention<ref>{{cite journal | title=Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Resolves Conflict from Distracting Stimuli by Boosting Attention toward Relevant Events |author1=D.H. Weissman |author2=A. Gopalakrishnan |author3=C.J. Hazlett |author4=M.G. Woldorff | journal=Cerebral Cortex | year=2005 | volume=15 | issue=2 | pages=229–237 | doi=10.1093/cercor/bhh125 | pmid=15238434| doi-access=free }}</ref> and behaviorally demanding cognitive tasks.<ref name="Medford N, Critchley HD. 535–549">{{cite journal | title=Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response | author=Medford N |author2=Critchley HD. | journal=Brain Struct Funct |date=June 2010 | volume=214 | issue=5–6 | pages=535–549 | doi=10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x | pmc=2886906 | pmid=20512367 }}</ref> It may be particularly important with regard to conscious, subjective emotional awareness. This region of the brain may play an important role in the initiation of motivated behavior.<ref name="Medford N, Critchley HD. 535–549"/> The [[subgenual cingulate]] is more active during both experimentally induced sadness and during depressive episodes.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Drevets|first1=Wayne C.|last2=Savitz|first2=Jonathan|last3=Trimble|first3=Michael|title=The Subgenual Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Mood Disorders|journal=CNS Spectrums|date=15 December 2016|volume=13|issue=8|pages=663–681|issn=1092-8529|pmc=2729429|pmid=18704022|doi=10.1017/s1092852900013754}}</ref>


===Other brain structures ===
===Other brain structures ===

*[[Basal ganglia]] – Basal ganglia are groups of nuclei found on either side of the thalamus. Basal ganglia play an important role in motivation,<ref>{{cite journal | title=The role of the basal ganglia in motivated behavior | author=Da Cunha C |author2=Gomez-A A |author3=Blaha CD. | journal=Rev Neurosci. | year=2012 | volume=23 | issue=5–6 | pages=747–67 | doi=10.1515/revneuro-2012-0063 | pmid=23079510 | s2cid=30904211 }}</ref> action selection and reward learning.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Mikhael|first1=John G.|last2=Bogacz|first2=Rafal|title=Learning Reward Uncertainty in the Basal Ganglia|journal=PLOS Computational Biology|date=2 September 2016|volume=12|issue=9|pages=e1005062|doi=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005062|pmid=27589489|issn=1553-7358|pmc=5010205|bibcode=2016PLSCB..12E5062M}}</ref>
* [[Basal ganglia]] – Basal ganglia are groups of nuclei found on either side of the thalamus. Basal ganglia play an important role in motivation<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Da Cunha |first=Claudio |last2=Gomez-A |first2=Alexander |last3=Blaha |first3=Charles D. |date=2012 |title=The role of the basal ganglia in motivated behavior |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/23079510 |journal=Reviews in the Neurosciences |volume=23 |issue=5-6 |pages=747–767 |doi=10.1515/revneuro-2012-0063 |issn=0334-1763 |pmid=23079510}}</ref>, action selection and reward learning<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Mikhael |first=John G. |last2=Bogacz |first2=Rafal |date=2016-09-02 |editor-last=Blackwell |editor-first=Kim T. |title=Learning Reward Uncertainty in the Basal Ganglia |url=https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005062 |journal=PLOS Computational Biology |language=en |volume=12 |issue=9 |pages=e1005062 |doi=10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005062 |issn=1553-7358 |pmc=PMC5010205 |pmid=27589489}}</ref>.
*[[Orbitofrontal cortex]] – The orbitofrontal cortex is a major structure involved in decision making and the influence by emotion on that decision.<ref name=2000_Bechara>{{cite journal|last=Bechara|first=Antoine|author2=H. Damasio|author3=A. Damasio|title=Emotion, Decision Making and the Orbitofrontal Cortex|journal=Cerebral Cortex|year=2000|volume=10|issue=3|pages=295–307|doi=10.1093/cercor/10.3.295|pmid=10731224|doi-access=free}}</ref>
* [[Orbitofrontal cortex]] – The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in decision making and helping us understand how emotions have influenced our decision making <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Bechara |first=A. |date=2000-03-01 |title=Emotion, Decision Making and the Orbitofrontal Cortex |url=https://academic.oup.com/cercor/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/cercor/10.3.295 |journal=Cerebral Cortex |volume=10 |issue=3 |pages=295–307 |doi=10.1093/cercor/10.3.295}}</ref>.
* [[Prefrontal cortex]] – The prefrontal cortex is the front of the brain, behind the forehead and above the eyes. It appears to play a critical role in the regulation of emotion and behavior by anticipating consequences. It may play an important role in delayed gratification by maintaining emotions over time and organizing behavior toward specific goals.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Davidson | first1 = R.J. | last2 = Sutton | first2 = S.K. | year = 1995 | title = Affective neuroscience: The emergence of a discipline | journal = Current Opinion in Neurobiology | volume = 5 | issue = 2| pages = 217–224 | doi=10.1016/0959-4388(95)80029-8| pmid = 7620310 | s2cid = 14639993 }}</ref>
* [[Prefrontal cortex]] – The prefrontal cortex is the front of the brain, behind the forehead and above the eyes. It plays a role in regulating emotion and behavior by anticipating consequences. The prefrontal cortex also plays an important role in delayed gratification by maintaining emotions over time and organizing behavior toward specific goals<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Davidson |first=Richard J |last2=Sutton |first2=Steven K |date=1995 |title=Affective neuroscience: the emergence of a discipline |url=https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/0959438895800298 |journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology |language=en |volume=5 |issue=2 |pages=217–224 |doi=10.1016/0959-4388(95)80029-8}}</ref>.
* Ventral [[striatum]] – The ventral striatum is a group of subcortical structures thought to play an important role in emotion and behavior. One part of the ventral striatum called the [[nucleus accumbens]] is thought to be involved in the experience of pleasure.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Kringelbach|first1=Morten L.|last2=Berridge|first2=Kent C.|title=The Functional Neuroanatomy of Pleasure and Happiness|journal=Discovery Medicine|date=15 December 2016|volume=9|issue=49|pages=579–587|issn=1539-6509|pmc=3008353|pmid=20587348}}</ref> Individuals with addictions experience increased activity in this area when they encounter the object of their [[Substance dependence|addiction]].
* Ventral [[striatum]] – The ventral striatum is a group of structures thought to play a role in emotion and behavior. An area of the ventral striatum known as the nucleus accumbens is involved in the experience of pleasure <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kringelbach |first=Morten L. |last2=Berridge |first2=Kent C. |date=2010 |title=The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/20587348 |journal=Discovery Medicine |volume=9 |issue=49 |pages=579–587 |issn=1944-7930 |pmc=3008353 |pmid=20587348}}</ref>. It is common for individuals with addictions to exhibit increased activity in this area when they are exposed to the object of their [[addiction]].
* [[Insular cortex|Insula]] – The insular cortex is thought to play a critical role in the bodily experience of emotion, as it is connected to other brain structures that regulate the body's autonomic functions (heart rate, breathing, digestion, etc.). The insula is implicated in empathy and awareness of emotion.<ref>{{cite journal|last1=Gu|first1=Xiaosi|last2=Hof|first2=Patrick R.|last3=Friston|first3=Karl J.|last4=Fan|first4=Jin|title=Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness|journal=The Journal of Comparative Neurology|date=15 October 2013|volume=521|issue=15|pages=3371–3388|doi=10.1002/cne.23368|issn=0021-9967|pmid=23749500|pmc=3999437}}</ref>
* [[Insular cortex|Insula]] – This area of the brain plays a significant role in bodily emotions due to its connections to other neural structures that control automatic functions such as heart rate, breathing, and digestion<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Rolls |first=Edmund T. |date=2016-12-01 |title=Functions of the anterior insula in taste, autonomic, and related functions |url=https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S027826261530004X |journal=Brain and Cognition |series=Food for thought: The functional and neural mechanisms of food perception and choice |language=en |volume=110 |pages=4–19 |doi=10.1016/j.bandc.2015.07.002 |issn=0278-2626}}</ref>. The insula is also implicated in empathy and awareness of emotion<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Gu |first=Xiaosi |last2=Hof |first2=Patrick R. |last3=Friston |first3=Karl J. |last4=Fan |first4=Jin |date=2013 |title=Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness: Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness |url=https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/cne.23368 |journal=Journal of Comparative Neurology |language=en |volume=521 |issue=15 |pages=3371–3388 |doi=10.1002/cne.23368 |pmc=PMC3999437 |pmid=23749500}}</ref>.
* [[Cerebellum]] – A "[[Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome]]" has been described.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Parvizi | last2 = Anderson | last3 = Damasio | last4 = Damasio | year = 2001 | title = Pathological laughing and crying: a link to the cerebellum | journal = Brain | volume = 124 | issue = 9| pages = 1708–19 | doi=10.1093/brain/124.9.1708 | pmid=11522574| doi-access = free }}</ref> Both neuroimaging studies as well as studies following pathological cerebellar lesions (such as a [[stroke]]) demonstrate that the cerebellum has a significant role in emotional regulation. Lesion studies<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Turner | first1 = Paradiso | last2 = Marvel | first2 = Pierson | last3 = Ponto | first3 = Boles | last4 = Hichwa | first4 = Robinson | last5 = Boles Ponto | first5 = L. L. | last6 = Hichwa | first6 = R. D. | last7 = Robinson | first7 = R. G. |date=March 2007 | title = The cerebellum and emotional experience| journal = Neuropsychologia | volume = 45 | issue = 6| pages = 1331–1341 | doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023 | pmid=17123557 | pmc=1868674}}</ref> have shown that cerebellar dysfunction can attenuate the experience of positive emotions. While these same studies do not show an attenuated response to frightening stimuli, the stimuli did not recruit structures that normally would be activated (such as the amygdala). Rather, alternative structures were activated, such as the [[ventromedial prefrontal cortex]], the anterior cingulate gyrus, and the insula. This may indicate that evolutionary pressure resulted in the development of the cerebellum as a redundant fear-mediating circuit to enhance survival. It may also indicate a regulatory role for the cerebellum in the neural response to rewarding stimuli, such as money,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Martin-Solch | first1 = C | last2 = Magyar | first2 = S | last3 = Kunig | first3 = G | last4 = Missimer | first4 = J | last5 = Schultz | first5 = W | last6 = Leenders | first6 = KL. | year = 2001 | title = Changes in brain activation associated with reward processing in smokers and nonsmokers. A positron emission tomography study | journal = Experimental Brain Research | volume = 139 | issue = 3| pages = 278–286 | doi=10.1007/s002210100751| pmid = 11545466 | s2cid = 29989526 }}</ref> drugs of abuse,<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Sell | first1 = LA | last2 = Morris | first2 = J | last3 = Bearn | first3 = J | last4 = Frackowiak | first4 = RS | last5 = Friston | first5 = KJ | last6 = Dolan | first6 = RJ. | year = 1999 | title = Activation of reward circuitry in human opiate addicts | journal = European Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 11 | issue = 3| pages = 1042–1048 | doi=10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00522.x| pmid = 10103096 | hdl = 21.11116/0000-0001-A04C-5 | s2cid = 12004748 | hdl-access = free }}</ref> and orgasm.<ref>{{cite journal | last1 = Holstege | first1 = G | last2 = Georgiadis | first2 = JR | last3 = Paans | first3 = AM | last4 = Meiners | first4 = LC | last5 = der Graaf | first5 = FH | last6 = Reinders | first6 = AA. | year = 2003 | title = Brain activation during human male ejaculation | journal = Journal of Neuroscience | volume = 23 | issue = 27| pages = 9185–9193 | pmid = 14534252 | pmc = 6740826 | doi = 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09185.2003 }}</ref>
* [[Cerebellum]] – A "[[Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome]]" has been described resulting in personality change and how the person shows emotions<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Parvizi |first=J. |date=2001-09-01 |title=Pathological laughter and crying: A link to the cerebellum |url=https://academic.oup.com/brain/article-lookup/doi/10.1093/brain/124.9.1708 |journal=Brain |volume=124 |issue=9 |pages=1708–1719 |doi=10.1093/brain/124.9.1708}}</ref>. Cerebellar dysfunction has been shown to decrease positive emotions during lesion studies<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Turner |first=Beth M. |last2=Paradiso |first2=Sergio |last3=Marvel |first3=Cherie L. |last4=Pierson |first4=Ronald |last5=Boles Ponto |first5=Laura L. |last6=Hichwa |first6=Richard D. |last7=Robinson |first7=Robert G. |date=2007-03-25 |title=The cerebellum and emotional experience |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/17123557 |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=45 |issue=6 |pages=1331–1341 |doi=10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023 |issn=0028-3932 |pmc=1868674 |pmid=17123557}}</ref>. Over the course of evolution, the cerebellum may have evolved into a circuit that helps reduce fear in order to enhance survival.The cerebellum may also play a regulatory role in the neural response to rewarding stimuli, such as money<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Martin-Sölch |first=C. |last2=Magyar |first2=S. |last3=Künig |first3=G. |last4=Missimer |first4=J. |last5=Schultz |first5=W. |last6=Leenders |first6=K. L. |date=2001 |title=Changes in brain activation associated with reward processing in smokers and nonsmokers. A positron emission tomography study |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/11545466 |journal=Experimental Brain Research |volume=139 |issue=3 |pages=278–286 |doi=10.1007/s002210100751 |issn=0014-4819 |pmid=11545466}}</ref>, addictive drugs<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Sell |first=L. A. |last2=Morris |first2=J. |last3=Bearn |first3=J. |last4=Frackowiak |first4=R. S. |last5=Friston |first5=K. J. |last6=Dolan |first6=R. J. |date=1999 |title=Activation of reward circuitry in human opiate addicts |url=https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10103096 |journal=The European Journal of Neuroscience |volume=11 |issue=3 |pages=1042–1048 |doi=10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00522.x |issn=0953-816X |pmid=10103096}}</ref>, and orgasm<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Zhou |first=Huan-Xiang |date=2008-11-05 |title=The debut of PMC Biophysics |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1757-5036-1-1 |journal=PMC Biophysics |volume=1 |issue=1 |doi=10.1186/1757-5036-1-1 |issn=1757-5036}}</ref>.
*Lateral prefrontal cortex
* [[Lateral prefrontal cortex]] - Using our emotions, the lateral prefrontal cortex is responsible for helping us reach our goals by suppressing harmful behaviors or selecting productive ones <ref>{{Citation |last=Barbas |first=Helen |title=Prefrontal Cortex Integration of Emotion and Cognition |date=2017 |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-56508-6_4 |work=The Prefrontal Cortex as an Executive, Emotional, and Social Brain |pages=51–76 |place=Tokyo |publisher=Springer Japan |isbn=978-4-431-56506-2 |access-date=2022-11-27 |last2=García-Cabezas |first2=Miguel Ángel}}</ref>.
*Primary sensorimotor cortex
* Primary [[Motor cortex|sensorimotor cortex]] - The somatosensory cortex is involved in each stage of emotional processing<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kropf |first=Erika |last2=Syan |first2=Sabrina K. |last3=Minuzzi |first3=Luciano |last4=Frey |first4=Benicio N. |date=2019 |title=From anatomy to function: the role of the somatosensory cortex in emotional regulation |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2018-0183 |journal=Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry |volume=41 |issue=3 |pages=261–269 |doi=10.1590/1516-4446-2018-0183 |issn=1809-452X}}</ref>. We use it to collect information that helps us in identifying and creating emotion, and then regulate that emotion once it has started.
*Temporal cortex
* [[Temporal lobe|Temporal cortex]] - This brain area is important in processing sound, speech, and language use, as well as helping us understand others faces and others emotions based on facial cues <ref>{{Cite journal |last=Goghari |first=V. M. |last2=MacDonald |first2=A. W. |last3=Sponheim |first3=S. R. |date=2010-05-19 |title=Temporal Lobe Structures and Facial Emotion Recognition in Schizophrenia Patients and Nonpsychotic Relatives |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq046 |journal=Schizophrenia Bulletin |volume=37 |issue=6 |pages=1281–1294 |doi=10.1093/schbul/sbq046 |issn=0586-7614}}</ref>. The temporal cortex is responsible for determining the quality and content of our emotional memories<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Kumfor |first=Fiona |last2=Irish |first2=Muireann |last3=Hodges |first3=John R. |last4=Piguet |first4=Olivier |date=2014-06-24 |title=Frontal and temporal lobe contributions to emotional enhancement of memory in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00225 |journal=Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience |volume=8 |doi=10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00225 |issn=1662-5153}}</ref>.
**Brainstem
* [[Brainstem]] - The brainstem is composed of three parts: ascending (sensory information), descending (motor information), and modulatory<ref>{{Cite journal |last=Venkatraman |first=Anand |last2=Edlow |first2=Brian L. |last3=Immordino-Yang |first3=Mary Helen |date=2017-03-09 |title=The Brainstem in Emotion: A Review |url=http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fnana.2017.00015 |journal=Frontiers in Neuroanatomy |volume=11 |doi=10.3389/fnana.2017.00015 |issn=1662-5129}}</ref>. The brainstem takes information from our environment (ascending) and creates a bodily response (descending) such as crying. The information from the environment and our body's responses to the information we receive is combined in the modulatory part of the brain stem and we are then able to label an emotion.


===Right hemisphere ===
===Right hemisphere ===

Revision as of 18:37, 1 December 2022

Emotions are thought to be related to activity in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and help us make decisions about our environment. Early stages of research on emotions and the brain was conducted by Paul Broca[1], James Papez[2], and Paul D. MacLean[3]. Their work suggests that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the limbic system. The limbic system is made up of the following brain structures:

Limbic system

Research has shown the limbic system is directly related to emotion, but there are other brain areas and structures that are important for producing and processing emotion[19].

Other brain structures

  • Basal ganglia – Basal ganglia are groups of nuclei found on either side of the thalamus. Basal ganglia play an important role in motivation[20], action selection and reward learning[21].
  • Orbitofrontal cortex – The orbitofrontal cortex is involved in decision making and helping us understand how emotions have influenced our decision making [22].
  • Prefrontal cortex – The prefrontal cortex is the front of the brain, behind the forehead and above the eyes. It plays a role in regulating emotion and behavior by anticipating consequences. The prefrontal cortex also plays an important role in delayed gratification by maintaining emotions over time and organizing behavior toward specific goals[23].
  • Ventral striatum – The ventral striatum is a group of structures thought to play a role in emotion and behavior. An area of the ventral striatum known as the nucleus accumbens is involved in the experience of pleasure [24]. It is common for individuals with addictions to exhibit increased activity in this area when they are exposed to the object of their addiction.
  • Insula – This area of the brain plays a significant role in bodily emotions due to its connections to other neural structures that control automatic functions such as heart rate, breathing, and digestion[25]. The insula is also implicated in empathy and awareness of emotion[26].
  • Cerebellum – A "Cerebellar Cognitive Affective Syndrome" has been described resulting in personality change and how the person shows emotions[27]. Cerebellar dysfunction has been shown to decrease positive emotions during lesion studies[28]. Over the course of evolution, the cerebellum may have evolved into a circuit that helps reduce fear in order to enhance survival.The cerebellum may also play a regulatory role in the neural response to rewarding stimuli, such as money[29], addictive drugs[30], and orgasm[31].
  • Lateral prefrontal cortex - Using our emotions, the lateral prefrontal cortex is responsible for helping us reach our goals by suppressing harmful behaviors or selecting productive ones [32].
  • Primary sensorimotor cortex - The somatosensory cortex is involved in each stage of emotional processing[33]. We use it to collect information that helps us in identifying and creating emotion, and then regulate that emotion once it has started.
  • Temporal cortex - This brain area is important in processing sound, speech, and language use, as well as helping us understand others faces and others emotions based on facial cues [34]. The temporal cortex is responsible for determining the quality and content of our emotional memories[35].
  • Brainstem - The brainstem is composed of three parts: ascending (sensory information), descending (motor information), and modulatory[36]. The brainstem takes information from our environment (ascending) and creates a bodily response (descending) such as crying. The information from the environment and our body's responses to the information we receive is combined in the modulatory part of the brain stem and we are then able to label an emotion.

Right hemisphere

The right hemisphere has been proposed as directly involved in emotion processing. Scientific theory regarding its role produced several models of emotional functioning. C. K. Mills was an early researcher who proposed a direct link between the right hemisphere and emotion processing, having observed decreased emotion processing in patients with lesions to the right hemisphere.[37][38] In the late 1980s to early 1990s neocortical structures were shown to have an involvement in emotion.[39] These findings led to the development of the right hemisphere hypothesis and the valence hypothesis.

Right hemisphere hypothesis

The right hemisphere hypothesis says that the right hemisphere of the brain is specialized for and processes expression and perception of emotion more effectively than the left hemisphere.[40] It has been linked with mental strategies that are nonverbal, synthetic, integrative, holistic, and gestaltic.[39] The right hemisphere is more in touch with subcortical systems of autonomic arousal and attention as demonstrated in patients that have increased spatial neglect when damage affects the right brain versus the left brain.[41] Right hemisphere pathologies have been linked with abnormal patterns of autonomic nervous system responses.[42] These findings would help signify the strong connection of the subcortical brain regions to the right hemisphere.

Valence hypothesis

The valence hypothesis acknowledges the right hemisphere's role in emotion, but asserts that it is mainly focused on the processing of negative emotions whereas the left hemisphere processes positive emotions. The two hemispheres have been the subject of much debate. One version states that the right hemisphere processes negative emotion leaving positive emotion to the left brain.[43] A second version suggests that the right hemisphere predominates in experiencing both positive and negative emotion.[44][45] More recently, the frontal lobe has been the focus of research, asserting that the frontal lobes of both hemispheres are involved in emotions, while the parietal and temporal lobes are involved in the processing of emotion.[46] Decreased right parietal lobe activity has been associated with depression[47] and increased right parietal lobe activity with anxiety arousal.[48] The increasing understanding of the different hemispheres has led to increasingly complicated models, all based on the original valence model.[49]

Cognitive neuroscience

Despite their interactions, the study of cognition until the late 1990s, excluded emotion and focused on non-emotional processes (e.g., memory, attention, perception, action, problem solving and mental imagery).[50] The study of the neural basis of non-emotional and emotional processes emerged as two separate fields: cognitive neuroscience and affective neuroscience. Emotional and non-emotional processes often involve overlapping neural and mental mechanisms.[51]

Cognitive neuroscience tasks in affective neuroscience research

Emotion go/no-go

The emotion go/no-go task has been used to study behavioral inhibition, particularly emotional modulation of this inhibition.[52] A derivation of the original go/no-go paradigm, this task involves a combination of affective "go cues", where the participant must rapidly make a motor response, and affective "no-go cues", where a response must be withheld. Because "go cues" are more common, the task measures a subject's ability to inhibit a response under different emotional conditions.[53]

The task is common in tests of emotion regulation, and is often paired with neuroimaging measures to localize relevant brain function in both healthy individuals and those with affective disorders.[52][54][55] For example, go/no-go studies converge with other methodology to implicate areas of the prefrontal cortex during inhibition of emotionally valenced stimuli.[56]

Emotional Stroop

The emotional Stroop task, an adaptation to the original Stroop, measures attentional bias to emotional stimuli.[57][58] Participants must name the ink color of presented words while ignoring the words' meanings.[59] In general, participants have more difficulty detaching attention from affectively valenced words, than neutral words.[60][61] This interference from valenced words is measured by the response latency in naming the color of neutral words as compared with emotional words.[58]

This task has been often used to test selective attention to threatening and other negatively valenced stimuli, most often in relation to psychopathology.[62] Disorder-specific attentional biases have been found for a variety of mental disorders.[62][63] For example, participants with spider phobia show a bias to spider-related words but not other negatively valenced words.[64] Similar findings have been attributed to threat words related to other anxiety disorders.[62] However, other studies have questioned these findings. In fact, anxious participants in some studies show the Stroop interference effect for both negative and positive words, when the words are matched for emotionality.[65][66] This means that the specificity effects for various disorders may be largely attributable to the semantic relation of the words to the concerns of the disorder, rather than their emotionality.[62]

Ekman 60 faces task

The Ekman faces task is used to measure emotion recognition of six basic emotions.[67][68] Black and white photographs of 10 actors (6 male, 4 female) are presented, with each actor displaying each emotion. Participants are usually asked to respond quickly with the name of the displayed emotion. The task is a common tool to study deficits in emotion regulation in patients with dementia, Parkinson's, and other cognitively degenerative disorders.[69] The task has been used to analyze recognition errors in disorders such as borderline personality disorder, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorder.[70][71][72]

Dot probe (emotion)

The emotional dot-probe paradigm is a task used to assess selective visual attention to and failure to detach attention from affective stimuli.[73][74] The paradigm begins with a fixation cross at the center of a screen. An emotional stimulus and a neutral stimulus appear side by side, after which a dot appears behind either the neutral stimulus (incongruent condition) or the affective stimulus (congruent condition). Participants are asked to indicate when they see this dot, and response latency is measured. Dots that appear on the same side of the screen as the image the participant was looking at will be identified more quickly. Thus, it is possible to discern which object the participant was attending to by subtracting the reaction time to respond to congruent versus incongruent trials.[73]

The best documented research with the dot probe paradigm involves attention to threat related stimuli, such as fearful faces, in individuals with anxiety disorders. Anxious individuals tend to respond more quickly to congruent trials, which may indicate vigilance to threat and/or failure to detach attention from threatening stimuli.[73][75] A specificity effect of attention has also been noted, with individuals attending selectively to threats related to their particular disorder. For example, those with social phobia selectively attend to social threats but not physical threats.[76] However, this specificity may be even more nuanced. Participants with obsessive-compulsive disorder symptoms initially show attentional bias to compulsive threat, but this bias is attenuated in later trials due to habituation to the threat stimuli.[77]

Fear potentiated startle

Fear-potentiated startle (FPS) has been utilized as a psychophysiological index of fear reaction in both animals and humans.[78] FPS is most often assessed through the magnitude of the eyeblink startle reflex, which can be measured by electromyography.[79] This eyeblink reflex is an automatic defensive reaction to an abrupt elicitor, making it an objective indicator of fear.[80] Typical FPS paradigms involve bursts of noise or abrupt flashes of light transmitted while an individual attends to a set of stimuli.[80] Startle reflexes have been shown to be modulated by emotion. For example, healthy participants tend to show enhanced startle responses while viewing negatively valenced images and attenuated startle while viewing positively valenced images, as compared with neutral images.[81][82]

The startle response to a particular stimulus is greater under conditions of threat.[83] A common example given to indicate this phenomenon is that one's startle response to a flash of light will be greater when walking in a dangerous neighborhood at night than it would under safer conditions. In laboratory studies, the threat of receiving shock is enough to potentiate startle, even without any actual shock.[84]

Fear potentiated startle paradigms are often used to study fear learning and extinction in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other anxiety disorders.[85][86][87] In fear conditioning studies, an initially neutral stimulus is repeatedly paired with an aversive one, borrowing from classical conditioning.[88] FPS studies have demonstrated that PTSD patients have enhanced startle responses during both danger cues and neutral/safety cues as compared with healthy participants.[88][89]

Learning

Affect plays many roles during learning. Deep, emotional attachment to a subject area allows a deeper understanding of the material and therefore, learning occurs and lasts.[90] The emotions evoked when reading in comparison to the emotions portrayed in the content affects comprehension. Someone who is feeling sad understands a sad passage better than someone feeling happy.[91] Therefore, a student's emotion plays an important role during the learning process.

Emotion can be embodied or perceived from words read on a page or in a facial expression. Neuroimaging studies using fMRI have demonstrated that the same area of the brain that is activated when feeling disgust is activated when observing another's disgust.[92] In a traditional learning environment, the teacher's facial expression can play a critical role in language acquisition. Showing a fearful facial expression when reading passages that contain fearful tones facilitates students learning of the meaning of certain vocabulary words and comprehension of the passage.[93]

Models

The neurobiological basis of emotion is still disputed.[94] The existence of basic emotions and their defining attributes represents a long lasting and yet unsettled issue in psychology.[94] The available research suggests that the neurobiological existence of basic emotions is still tenable and heuristically seminal, pending some reformulation.[94]

Basic Emotions

These approaches hypothesize that emotion categories (including happiness, sadness, fear, anger, and disgust) are biologically basic.[95][96] In this view, emotions are inherited, biologically based modules that cannot be separated into more basic psychological components.[95][96][97] Models following this approach hypothesize that all mental states belonging to a single emotional category can be consistently and specifically localized to either a single brain region or a defined network of brain regions.[96][98] Each basic emotion category also shares other universal characteristics: distinct facial behavior, physiology, subjective experience and accompanying thoughts and memories.[95]

Psychological constructionist approaches

This approach to emotion hypothesizes that emotions like happiness, sadness, fear, anger and disgust (and many others) are constructed mental states that occur when brain systems work together.[99] In this view, networks of brain regions underlie psychological operations (e.g., language, attention, etc.) that interact to produce emotion, perception, and cognition.[100] One psychological operation critical for emotion is the network of brain regions that underlie valence (feeling pleasant/unpleasant) and arousal (feeling activated and energized).[99] Emotions emerge when neural systems underlying different psychological operations interact (not just those involved in valence and arousal), producing distributed patterns of activation across the brain. Because emotions emerge from more basic components, heterogeneity affects each emotion category; for example, a person can experience many different kinds of fear, which feel differently, and which correspond to different neural patterns in the brain.[101]

Meta-analyses

A meta-analysis is a statistical approach to synthesizing results across multiple studies. Included studies investigated healthy, unmedicated adults and that used subtraction analysis to examine brain areas that were more active during emotional processing than during a neutral (control) condition.

Phan et al. 2002

In the first neuroimaging meta-analysis of emotion, Phan et al. (2002) analyzed the results of 55 peer reviewed studies between January 1990 and December 2000 to determine if the emotions of fear, sadness, disgust, anger, and happiness were consistently associated with activity in specific brain regions. All studies used fMRI or PET techniques to investigate higher-order mental processing of emotion (studies of low-order sensory or motor processes were excluded). The authors’ tabulated the number of studies that reported activation in specific brain regions. For each brain region, statistical chi-squared analysis was conducted. Two regions showed a statistically significant association. In the amygdala, 66% of studies inducing fear reported activity in this region, as compared to ~20% of studies inducing happiness, ~15% of studies inducing sadness (with no reported activations for anger or disgust). In the subcallosal cingulate, 46% of studies inducing sadness reported activity in this region, as compared to ~20% inducing happiness and ~20% inducing anger. This pattern of clear discriminability between emotion categories was in fact rare, with other patterns occurring in limbic regions, paralimbic regions, and uni/heteromodal regions. Brain regions implicated across discrete emotion included the basal ganglia (~60% of studies inducing happiness and ~60% of studies inducing disgust reported activity in this region) and medial prefrontal cortex (happiness ~60%, anger ~55%, sadness ~40%, disgust ~40%, and fear ~30%).[102]

Murphy et al. 2003

Murphy, et al. 2003 analyzed 106 peer reviewed studies published between January 1994 and December 2001 to examine the evidence for regional specialization of discrete emotions (fear, disgust, anger, happiness and sadness) across a larger set of studies. Studies included in the meta-analysis measured activity in the whole brain and regions of interest (activity in individual regions of particular interest to the study). 3-D Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS3) statistics were used to compare rough spatial distributions of 3-D activation patterns to determine if statistically significant activations were specific to particular brain regions for all emotional categories. This pattern of consistently activated, regionally specific activations was identified in four brain regions: amygdala with fear (~40% of studies), insula with disgust (~70%), globus pallidus with disgust (~70%), and lateral orbitofrontal cortex with anger (80%). Other regions showed different patterns of activation across categories. For example, both the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex showed consistent activity across emotions (happiness ~50%, sadness ~50%, anger ~ 40%, fear ~30%, and disgust ~ 20%).[103]

Barrett et al. 2006

Barrett, et al. 2006 examined 161 studies published between 1990 and 2001. The authors compared the consistency and specificity of prior meta-analytic findings specific to each notional basic emotion. Consistent neural patterns were defined by brain regions showing increased activity for a specific emotion (relative to a neutral control condition), regardless of the method of induction used (for example, visual vs. auditory cue). Specific neural patterns were defined as separate circuits for one emotion vs. the other emotions (for example, the fear circuit must be discriminable from the anger circuit, although both may include common brain regions). In general, the results supported Phan et al. and Murphy et al., but not specificity. Consistency was determined through the comparison of chi-squared analyses that revealed whether the proportion of studies reporting activation during one emotion was significantly higher than the proportion of studies reporting activation during the other emotions. Specificity was determined through the comparison of emotion-category brain-localizations by contrasting activations in key regions that were specific to particular emotions. Increased amygdala activation during fear was the most consistently reported across induction methods (but not specific). Both meta-analyses associated the anterior cingulate cortex with sadness, although this finding was less consistent (across induction methods) and was not specific. Both meta-analyses found that disgust was associated with the basal ganglia, but these findings were neither consistent nor specific. Neither consistent nor specific activity was observed across the meta-analyses for anger or happiness. This meta-analysis introduced the concept of the basic, irreducible elements of emotional life as dimensions such as approach and avoidance.[99]

Kober et al. 2008

Kober, et al. 2008 reviewed 162 neuroimaging studies published between 1990–2005 to determine if groups of brain regions showed consistent activation patterns while experiencing an emotion directly and (indirectly) as experienced by another. This analysis used multilevel kernel density analysis (MKDA) to examine fMRI and PET studies, a technique that prevents single studies from dominating the results (particularly if they report multiple nearby peaks) and that enables studies involving more participants to exert more influence upon the results. MKDA was used to establish a neural reference space that includes the set of regions showing consistent increases across all studies.[104] This neural reference space was partitioned into functional groups of brain regions showing similar activation patterns by using multivariate techniques to determine co-activation patterns and then using data-reduction techniques to define the functional groupings, resulting in six groups. The authors discussed each functional group in terms of more basic psychological operations.

Groups
Group Regions Notes
Core limbic left amygdala, hypothalamus, periaqueductal gray/thalamus regions, and amygdala/ventral striatum/ventral globus pallidus/thalamus regions Integrative emotional center that plays a general role in evaluating affective significance.
Lateral Paralimbic ventral anterior insula/frontal operculum/right temporal pole/ posterior orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior insula/ posterior orbitofrontal cortex, the ventral anterior insula/ temporal cortex/ orbitofrontal cortex junction, the midinsula/ dorsal putamen, and the ventral striatum /mid insula/ left hippocampus Plays a role in motivation, contributing to the general valuation of stimuli and particularly in reward.
Medial Prefrontal Cortex dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex, and rostral dorsal anterior cingulate cortex Plays a role in both the generation and regulation of emotion.
Cognitive/ Motor Network right frontal operculum, the right interior frontal gyrus, and the pre-supplementray motor area/ left interior frontal gyrus, regions Not specific to emotion, but instead appear to play a more general role in information processing and cognitive control.
Occipital/ Visual Association V8 and V4 areas of the primary visual cortex, the medial temporal lobe, and the lateral occipital cortex
Medial Posterior posterior cingulate cortex and area V1 of the primary visual cortex

The authors suggest that these regions play a joint role in visual processing and attention to emotional stimuli.[105]

Vytal et al. 2010

Vytal, et al. 2010 examined 83 neuroimaging studies published between 1993–2008 to examine whether neuroimaging evidence supports biologically discrete, basic emotions (i.e. fear, anger, disgust, happiness, and sadness). Consistency analyses identified brain regions associated with individual emotions. Discriminability analyses identified brain regions that were differentially active under contrasting pairs of emotions. This meta-analysis examined PET or fMRI studies that reported whole brain analyses identifying significant activations for at least one of the five emotions relative to a neutral or control condition. The authors used activation likelihood estimation (ALE) to perform spatially sensitive, voxel-wise (sensitive to the spatial properties of voxels) statistical comparisons across studies. This technique allows for direct statistical comparison between activation maps associated with each discrete emotion. Thus, discriminability between the five discrete emotion categories was assessed on a more precise spatial scale than in prior meta-analyses.

Consistency was first assessed by comparing the cross-study ALE map for each emotion to ALE maps generated by random permutations. Discriminability was assessed by pair-wise contrasts of emotion maps. Consistent and discriminable activation patterns were observed for the five categories.

Associations[106]
Emotion Peak Regions
Happiness right superior temporal gyrus, left rostral anterior cingulate cortex 9 regional brain clusters
Sadness left medial frontal gyrus 35 clusters - especially, left medial frontal gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, and right inferior frontal gyrus
Anger left inferior frontal gyrus 13 clusters - bilateral inferior frontal gyrus, and in right parahippocampal gyrus
Fear left amygdala 11 clusters - left amygdala and left putamen
Disgust right insula/ right inferior frontal gyrus 16 clusters - right putamen and the left insula.

Lindquist et al. 2012

Lindquist, et al. reviewed 91 PET and fMRI studies published between January 1990 and December 2007. The studies used induction methods that elicit emotion experience or emotion perception of fear, sadness, disgust, anger, and happiness. The goal was to compare basic emotions approaches with psychological constructionist approaches. A MKDA[104] transformed the individual peak into a neural reference space. The density analysis was then used to identify voxels with more consistent activations for a specific emotion category than all other emotions. Chi-squared analysis was used to create statistical maps that indicated whether each previously identified and consistently active region was more frequently activated in studies of each emotion category than average, regardless of activations elsewhere in the brain. Chi-squared analysis and density analysis both defined functionally consistent and selective regions (regions that showed a more consistent activity increase) for one emotion category. Thus, a selective region could present increased activations to multiple emotions, as long as the response to one emotion was relatively stronger.

A series of logistic regressions were performed to identify regions that while consistent and selective to an emotion were additionally specific to that emotion. Specificity was defined as showing increased activations for only one emotional category. Strong support for basic emotions was defined as evidence that brain areas respond to only one emotional category. Strong support for the constructionist approach was defined as evidence that psychological operations consistently occur across many brain regions and multiple emotional categories.

The results indicated that many brain regions demonstrated consistent and selective activations in the experience or perception of one emotion category. Consistent with constructionist models, however, no region demonstrated functional specificity for the emotions of fear, disgust, happiness, sadness or anger.

The authors proposed different roles for the brain regions that have traditionally been associated with only one emotion category. The authors propose that the amygdala, anterior insula, orbitofrontal cortex each contribute to "core affect", which are basic feelings that are pleasant or unpleasant with some level of arousal.

Core affects
Region Role
Amygdala indicating whether external sensory information is motivationally salient, novel and/oor evokes uncertainty
Anterior insula represents core affective feelings in awareness across emotion categories, driven largely by body sensations
Orbitofrontal cortex functions as a site for integrating sensory information from the body and the world to guide behavior

Closely related to core affect, the authors propose that the anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex play vital roles in attention. The anterior cingulate supports the use of sensory information for directing attention and motor responses during response selection while the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex supporting executive attention. In many psychological construction approaches, emotions relate an individual's situation in the world to internal body states, referred to as "conceptualization". The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus were consistently active in this context: regions that play an important role conceptualizing are also involved in simulating previous experience (e.g. knowledge, memory). Language is also central to conceptualizing, and regions that support language, including ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, were also consistently active across studies of emotion experience and perception.[100]

See also

References

  1. ^ Broca, Paul (1878). "Anatomie comparée des circonvolutions cérébrales: le grand lobe limbique". Rev. Anthropology. 1: 385–498.
  2. ^ PAPEZ, JAMES W. (1937-10-01). "A PROPOSED MECHANISM OF EMOTION". Archives of Neurology And Psychiatry. 38 (4): 725. doi:10.1001/archneurpsyc.1937.02260220069003. ISSN 0096-6754.
  3. ^ MacLean, Paul D. (1952). "Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (Visceral brain)". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 4 (4): 407–418. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4. ISSN 0013-4694.
  4. ^ LeDoux, Joseph E. (1995). "Emotion: Clues from the Brain". Annual Review of Psychology. 46 (1): 209–235. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.46.020195.001233. ISSN 0066-4308.
  5. ^ Breiter, Hans C; Etcoff, Nancy L; Whalen, Paul J; Kennedy, William A; Rauch, Scott L; Buckner, Randy L; Strauss, Monica M; Hyman, Steven E; Rosen, Bruce R (1996). "Response and Habituation of the Human Amygdala during Visual Processing of Facial Expression". Neuron. 17 (5): 875–887. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6.
  6. ^ Sherman, S. (2006). "Thalamus". Scholarpedia. 1 (9): 1583. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1583. ISSN 1941-6016.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  7. ^ Steriade, M; Llinás, R R (1988). "The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay". Physiological Reviews. 68 (3): 649–742. doi:10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649. ISSN 0031-9333.
  8. ^ The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience. Jorge Armony, Patrik Vuilleumier. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2013. ISBN 978-1-107-31401-6. OCLC 825767970.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  9. ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2022). "The hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and episodic and semantic memory". Progress in Neurobiology. 217: 102334. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102334.
  10. ^ Fischer, Håkan; Wright, Christopher I; Whalen, Paul J; McInerney, Sean C; Shin, Lisa M; Rauch, Scott L (2003). "Brain habituation during repeated exposure to fearful and neutral faces: A functional MRI study". Brain Research Bulletin. 59 (5): 387–392. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00940-1.
  11. ^ Modi, Shilpi; Trivedi, Richa; Singh, Kavita; Kumar, Pawan; Rathore, Ram K.S.; Tripathi, Rajendra P.; Khushu, Subash (2013). "Individual differences in trait anxiety are associated with white matter tract integrity in fornix and uncinate fasciculus: Preliminary evidence from a DTI based tractography study". Behavioural Brain Research. 238: 188–192. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.10.007.
  12. ^ Vann, Seralynne D. (2010). "Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory". Neuropsychologia. 48 (8): 2316–2327. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.10.019.
  13. ^ Imai, Takeshi (2014). "Construction of functional neuronal circuitry in the olfactory bulb". Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 35: 180–188. doi:10.1016/j.semcdb.2014.07.012. ISSN 1084-9521.
  14. ^ Tong, Michelle T.; Peace, Shane T.; Cleland, Thomas A. (2014-07-07). "Properties and mechanisms of olfactory learning and memory". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00238. ISSN 1662-5153.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  15. ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2019), "The cingulate cortex and limbic systems for action, emotion, and memory", Handbook of Clinical Neurology, vol. 166, Elsevier, pp. 23–37, doi:10.1016/b978-0-444-64196-0.00002-9, ISBN 978-0-444-64196-0, retrieved 2022-11-27
  16. ^ Weissman, D.H. (2004-07-06). "Dorsal Anterior Cingulate Cortex Resolves Conflict from Distracting Stimuli by Boosting Attention toward Relevant Events". Cerebral Cortex. 15 (2): 229–237. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh125. ISSN 1460-2199.
  17. ^ Medford, Nick; Critchley, Hugo D. (2010). "Conjoint activity of anterior insular and anterior cingulate cortex: awareness and response". Brain Structure and Function. 214 (5–6): 535–549. doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0265-x. ISSN 1863-2653. PMC 2886906. PMID 20512367.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  18. ^ Drevets, Wayne C.; Savitz, Jonathan; Trimble, Michael (2008). "The Subgenual Anterior Cingulate Cortex in Mood Disorders". CNS Spectrums. 13 (8): 663–681. doi:10.1017/S1092852900013754. ISSN 1092-8529. PMC 2729429. PMID 18704022.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  19. ^ Dalgleish, Tim (2004). "The emotional brain". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 5 (7): 583–589. doi:10.1038/nrn1432. ISSN 1471-003X.
  20. ^ Da Cunha, Claudio; Gomez-A, Alexander; Blaha, Charles D. (2012). "The role of the basal ganglia in motivated behavior". Reviews in the Neurosciences. 23 (5–6): 747–767. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2012-0063. ISSN 0334-1763. PMID 23079510.
  21. ^ Mikhael, John G.; Bogacz, Rafal (2016-09-02). Blackwell, Kim T. (ed.). "Learning Reward Uncertainty in the Basal Ganglia". PLOS Computational Biology. 12 (9): e1005062. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005062. ISSN 1553-7358. PMC 5010205. PMID 27589489.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  22. ^ Bechara, A. (2000-03-01). "Emotion, Decision Making and the Orbitofrontal Cortex". Cerebral Cortex. 10 (3): 295–307. doi:10.1093/cercor/10.3.295.
  23. ^ Davidson, Richard J; Sutton, Steven K (1995). "Affective neuroscience: the emergence of a discipline". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 5 (2): 217–224. doi:10.1016/0959-4388(95)80029-8.
  24. ^ Kringelbach, Morten L.; Berridge, Kent C. (2010). "The functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness". Discovery Medicine. 9 (49): 579–587. ISSN 1944-7930. PMC 3008353. PMID 20587348.
  25. ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (2016-12-01). "Functions of the anterior insula in taste, autonomic, and related functions". Brain and Cognition. Food for thought: The functional and neural mechanisms of food perception and choice. 110: 4–19. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2015.07.002. ISSN 0278-2626.
  26. ^ Gu, Xiaosi; Hof, Patrick R.; Friston, Karl J.; Fan, Jin (2013). "Anterior insular cortex and emotional awareness: Anterior Insular Cortex and Emotional Awareness". Journal of Comparative Neurology. 521 (15): 3371–3388. doi:10.1002/cne.23368. PMC 3999437. PMID 23749500.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: PMC format (link)
  27. ^ Parvizi, J. (2001-09-01). "Pathological laughter and crying: A link to the cerebellum". Brain. 124 (9): 1708–1719. doi:10.1093/brain/124.9.1708.
  28. ^ Turner, Beth M.; Paradiso, Sergio; Marvel, Cherie L.; Pierson, Ronald; Boles Ponto, Laura L.; Hichwa, Richard D.; Robinson, Robert G. (2007-03-25). "The cerebellum and emotional experience". Neuropsychologia. 45 (6): 1331–1341. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.023. ISSN 0028-3932. PMC 1868674. PMID 17123557.
  29. ^ Martin-Sölch, C.; Magyar, S.; Künig, G.; Missimer, J.; Schultz, W.; Leenders, K. L. (2001). "Changes in brain activation associated with reward processing in smokers and nonsmokers. A positron emission tomography study". Experimental Brain Research. 139 (3): 278–286. doi:10.1007/s002210100751. ISSN 0014-4819. PMID 11545466.
  30. ^ Sell, L. A.; Morris, J.; Bearn, J.; Frackowiak, R. S.; Friston, K. J.; Dolan, R. J. (1999). "Activation of reward circuitry in human opiate addicts". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 11 (3): 1042–1048. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00522.x. ISSN 0953-816X. PMID 10103096.
  31. ^ Zhou, Huan-Xiang (2008-11-05). "The debut of PMC Biophysics". PMC Biophysics. 1 (1). doi:10.1186/1757-5036-1-1. ISSN 1757-5036.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  32. ^ Barbas, Helen; García-Cabezas, Miguel Ángel (2017), "Prefrontal Cortex Integration of Emotion and Cognition", The Prefrontal Cortex as an Executive, Emotional, and Social Brain, Tokyo: Springer Japan, pp. 51–76, ISBN 978-4-431-56506-2, retrieved 2022-11-27
  33. ^ Kropf, Erika; Syan, Sabrina K.; Minuzzi, Luciano; Frey, Benicio N. (2019). "From anatomy to function: the role of the somatosensory cortex in emotional regulation". Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry. 41 (3): 261–269. doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2018-0183. ISSN 1809-452X.
  34. ^ Goghari, V. M.; MacDonald, A. W.; Sponheim, S. R. (2010-05-19). "Temporal Lobe Structures and Facial Emotion Recognition in Schizophrenia Patients and Nonpsychotic Relatives". Schizophrenia Bulletin. 37 (6): 1281–1294. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbq046. ISSN 0586-7614.
  35. ^ Kumfor, Fiona; Irish, Muireann; Hodges, John R.; Piguet, Olivier (2014-06-24). "Frontal and temporal lobe contributions to emotional enhancement of memory in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00225. ISSN 1662-5153.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  36. ^ Venkatraman, Anand; Edlow, Brian L.; Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen (2017-03-09). "The Brainstem in Emotion: A Review". Frontiers in Neuroanatomy. 11. doi:10.3389/fnana.2017.00015. ISSN 1662-5129.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  37. ^ Mills, C. K. (1912a). "The cerebral mechanism of emotional expression". Transactions of the College of Physicians of Philadelphia. 34: 381–390.
  38. ^ Mills, C. K. (1912b). "The cortical representation of emotion, with a discussion of some points in the general nervous mechanism of expression in its relation to organic nervous disease and insanity". Proceedings of the American Medico-Psychological Association. 19: 297–300.
  39. ^ a b Borod, J. C. (1992). "Intel-hemispheric and intrahemispheric control of emotion: A focus on unilateral brain damage". Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 60 (3): 339–348. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.60.3.339. PMID 1619088.
  40. ^ Borod, J. C., Koff, E., & Caron, H. S. (1983). Right hemispheric specialization for the expression and appreciation of emotion: A focus on the face. In E. Perecman (Ed.), Cognitive functions in the right hemisphere (pp. 83-110). New York: Academic Press.
  41. ^ Heilman, K. M. (February 1982). Discussant comments. In J. C. Borod & R. Buck (Chairs), Asymmetries in facial expression: Method and meaning. Symposium conducted at the International Neuropsychological Society, Pittsburgh, PA.
  42. ^ Yokoyama, K.; Jennings, R.; Ackles, P.; Hood, B. S.; Boiler, F. (1987). "Lack of heart rate changes during an attention-demanding task after right hemisphere lesions". Neurology. 37 (4): 624–630. doi:10.1212/wnl.37.4.624. PMID 3561774. S2CID 38096461.
  43. ^ Silberman, E. K.; Weingartner, H. (1986). "Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion". Brain and Cognition. 5 (3): 322–353. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(86)90035-7. PMID 3530287. S2CID 2636348.
  44. ^ Fox, N. A. (1991). "If it's not left, it's right". American Psychologist. 46 (8): 863–872. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.46.8.863. PMID 1928939.
  45. ^ Davidson, R.; Ekman, P.; Saron, C. D.; Senulis, J. A.; Friesen, W V (1990). "Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology I". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (2): 330–341. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330. PMID 2319445.
  46. ^ Borod, J.C.; Cicero, B.A.; Obler, L.K.; Welkowitz, J.; Erhan, H.M.; Santschi, C.; et al. (1998). "Right hemisphere emotional perception: Evidence across multiple channels". Neuropsychology. 12 (3): 446–458. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.446. PMID 9673999.
  47. ^ Levin, RL; Heller, W; Mohanty, A; Herrington, JD; Miller, GA. (2007). "Cognitive deficits in depression and functional specificity of regional brain activity". Cognitive Therapy and Research. 31 (2): 211–233. doi:10.1007/s10608-007-9128-z. S2CID 22374128.
  48. ^ Engels, AS; Heller, W; Mohanty, A; Herrington, JD; Banich, MT; Webb, AG; Miller, GA (2007). "Specificity of regional brain activity in anxiety types during emotion processing". Psychophysiology. 44 (3): 352–363. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00518.x. PMID 17433094.
  49. ^ Spielberg, J.; Stewart, J; Levin, R.; Miller, G.; Heller, W. (2008). "Prefrontal Cortex, Emotion, and Approach/Withdrawal Motivation". Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2 (1): 135–153. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00064.x. PMC 2889703. PMID 20574551.
  50. ^ Cacioppo, J.T.; Gardner, W.L. (1999). "Emotion". Annual Review of Psychology. 50: 191–214. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.191. PMID 10074678.
  51. ^ Davidson, R.J. (2000). "Cognitive neuroscience needs affective neuroscience (and vice versa)". Brain and Cognition. 42 (1): 89–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.487.9015. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1170. PMID 10739607. S2CID 22183667.
  52. ^ a b Drevets, W.C.; Raichle, M.E. (1998). "Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between emotion and cognition". Cognition and Emotion. 12 (3): 353–385. doi:10.1080/026999398379646.
  53. ^ Schulz, K.P.; Fan, J.; Magidina, O.; Marks, D.J.; Hahn, B.; Halperin, J.M. (2007). "Does the emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition? Convergence with measures on a non-emotional analog". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (2): 151–160. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.12.001. PMC 2562664. PMID 17207962.
  54. ^ Elliott, R.; Ogilvie, A.; Rubinsztein, J.S.; Calderon, G.; Dolan, R.J.; Sahakian, B.J. (2004). "Abnormal ventral frontal response during performance of an affective go/no go task in patients with mania". Biological Psychiatry. 55 (12): 1163–1170. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.03.007. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A247-8. PMID 15184035. S2CID 6511699.
  55. ^ Elliott, R.; Rubinsztein, J.S.; Sahakian, B.J.; Dolan, R.J. (2000). "Selective attention to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: An fMRI study". Brain Imaging. 11 (8): 1739–1744. doi:10.1097/00001756-200006050-00028. PMID 10852235. S2CID 16872952.
  56. ^ Hare, T.A.; Tottenham, N.; Davidson, M.C.; Glover, G.H.; Casey, B.J. (2005). "Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation". Biological Psychiatry. 57 (6): 624–632. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038. PMID 15780849. S2CID 17690328.
  57. ^ Stroop, J.R. (1935). "Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 18 (6): 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-5ADB-7.
  58. ^ a b Epp, A.M.; Dobson, K.S.; Dozois, D.J.A.; Frewen, P.A. (2012). "A systematic meta-analysis of the stroop task in depression". Clinical Psychology Review. 32 (4): 316–328. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2012.02.005. PMID 22459792.
  59. ^ Pratto, F.; John, O. P. (1991). "Automatic vigilance: The attention grabbing power of negative social information". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 61 (3): 380–391. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380. PMID 1941510.
  60. ^ Wentura, D.; Rothermund, K.; Bak, P. (2000). "Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of approach and avoidance-related social information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (6): 1024–1037. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1024. PMID 10870906.
  61. ^ Williams, J.M.; Broadbent, K. (1986). "Distraction by emotional stimuli: use of a Stroop task with suicide attempters". British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 25 (2): 101–110. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1986.tb00678.x. PMID 3730646.
  62. ^ a b c d Williams, M.G.; Matthews, A.; MacLeod, C. (1996). "The emotional stroop task and psychopathology". Psychological Bulletin. 120 (1): 3–24. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3. PMID 8711015.
  63. ^ Gotlib, I.H., Roberts, J.E., & Gilboa, E. (1996). Cognitive interference in depression. in I.G. Sarason, G.R. Pierce, & B.R. Sarason (Eds.), Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc., Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 347–377.
  64. ^ Watts, E N.; McKenna, E P.; Sharrock, R.; Trezise, L. (1986). "Colour naming of phobia-related words". British Journal of Psychology. 77: 97–108. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb01985.x. PMID 2869817.
  65. ^ Martin, M.; Williams, R. M.; Clark, D. M. (1991). "Does anxiety lead to selective processing of threat-related information?". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 29 (2): 147–160. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(91)90043-3. PMID 2021377.
  66. ^ Mogg, K.; Mathews, A. M.; Weinman, J. (1989). "Selective processing of threat cues in anxiety states: A replication". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 27 (4): 317–323. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(89)90001-6. PMID 2775141.
  67. ^ Diehl-Schmid, J.; Pohl, C.; Ruprecht, C.; Wagenpfeil, S.; Foerstl, H.; Kurz, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID 17360152.
  68. ^ Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Consulting Psychologists Press: Palo Alto, CA.
  69. ^ Diehl-Schmida, J.; Pohla, C.; Ruprechta, C.; Wagenpfeilb, S.; Foerstla, H.; Kurza, A. (2007). "The Ekman 60 faces test as a diagnostic instrument in frontotemporal dementia". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 22 (4): 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.024. PMID 17360152.
  70. ^ Ibarretxe-Bilbao, N.; Junque, C.; Tolosa, E.; Marti, M.; Valldeoriola, F.; Bargalloand, N.; Zareio, M. (2009). "Neuroanatomical correlates of impaired decision-making and facial emotion recognition in early Parkinson's disease". European Journal of Neuroscience. 30 (6): 1162–1171. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06892.x. PMID 19735293. S2CID 413809.
  71. ^ Soeiro-de-Souza, M.G.; Bio, D.S.; David, D.P.; Santos, D.R.; Kerr, D.S.; Moreno, R.A.; Machado-Vieira, Rodrigo; Moreno, Ricardo Albeto (2012). "COMT Met (158) modulates facial emotion recognition in bipolar I disorder mood episodes". Journal of Affective Disorders. 136 (3): 370–376. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2011.11.021. PMID 22222175.
  72. ^ Ebert, A.; Haussleiter, I.S.; Juckel, G.; Brune, M.; Roser, P. (2012). "Impaired facial emotion recognition in a ketamine model of psychosis". Psychiatry Research. 200 (2–3): 724–727. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.034. PMID 22776754. S2CID 43744551.
  73. ^ a b c Koster, E.H.W.; Crombez, G.; Verschuere, B.; De Houwer, J. (2004). "Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 42 (10): 1183–1192. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.001. PMID 15350857.
  74. ^ MacLeod, C.; Mathews, A.; Tata, P. (1986). "Attentional bias in emotional disorders". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 95 (1): 15–20. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.95.1.15. PMID 3700842.
  75. ^ Mogg, K.; Bradley, B. P. (1998). "A cognitive–motivational analysis of anxiety". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 36 (9): 809–848. doi:10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00063-1. PMID 9701859.
  76. ^ Asmundson, G.J.G.; Stein, M.B. (1994). "Selective processing of social threat in patients with generalized social phobia: Evaluation using a dot-probe paradigm". Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 8 (2): 107–117. doi:10.1016/0887-6185(94)90009-4.
  77. ^ Amir, N.; Naimi, S.; Morrison, A.S. (2009). "Attenuation of attention bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 47 (2): 153–157. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.020. PMC 2662360. PMID 19046576.
  78. ^ Davis, M. (1986). "Pharmacological and anatomical analysis of fear conditioning using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Behavioral Neuroscience. 100 (6): 814–824. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.100.6.814. PMID 3545257.
  79. ^ Baskin-Sommers, A.R.; Vitale, J.E.; MacCoon, D.; Newman, J.P. (2012). "Assessing emotion sensitivity in female offenders with borderline personality symptoms: Results from a fear-potentiated startle paradigm". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (2): 477–483. doi:10.1037/a0026753. PMC 3358451. PMID 22250659.
  80. ^ a b Vaidyanathan, U.; Patrick, C.J.; Cuthbert, B.N. (2009). "Linking dimensional models of internalizing psychopathology to neurobiological systems: Affect-modulated startle as an indicator of fear and distress disorders and affiliated traits". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (6): 909–942. doi:10.1037/a0017222. PMC 2776729. PMID 19883142.
  81. ^ Lang, P. J.; Bradley, M. M.; Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). "Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex". Psychological Review. 97 (3): 377–395. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.97.3.377. PMID 2200076.
  82. ^ Vrana, S. R.; Spence, E. L.; Lang, P. J. (1988). "The startle probe response: A new measure of emotion?". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 97 (4): 487–491. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.97.4.487. PMID 3204235.
  83. ^ Norrholm, S.D.; Anderson, K.M.; Olin, I.W.; Jovanovic, T.; Kwon, C.; Warren, Victor T.; Bradley, B.; Bosshardt, Lauren; Sabree, Justin; Duncan, Erica J.; Rothbaum, Barbara O.; Bradley, Bekh (2011). "Versatility of fear-potentiated startle paradigms for assessing human conditioned fear extinction and return of fear". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 5: 77. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2011.00077. PMC 3221285. PMID 22125516.
  84. ^ Grillon, C.; Ameli, R.; Woods, S. W.; Merikangas, K.; Davis, M. (1991). "Fear-potentiated startle in humans: Effects of anticipatory anxiety on the acoustic blink reflex". Psychophysiology. 28 (5): 588–595. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1991.tb01999.x. PMID 1758934.
  85. ^ Norrholm, S.; Jovanovic, Tanja (2011). "Translational fear inhibition models as indices of trauma-related psychopathology". Current Psychiatry Reviews. 7 (3): 194–204. doi:10.2174/157340011797183193.
  86. ^ Norrholm, S. D.; Jovanovic, T.; Olin, I. W.; Sands, L.; Karapanou, I.; Bradley, B.; Ressler, K. J. (2010). "Fear extinction in traumatized civilians with posttraumatic stress disorder: relation to symptom severity". Biological Psychiatry. 69 (6): 556–563. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.013. PMC 3052965. PMID 21035787.
  87. ^ Lang, P. J.; McTeague, L. M. (2009). "The anxiety disorder spectrum: Fear imagery, physiological reactivity, and differential diagnosis". Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 22 (1): 5–25. doi:10.1080/10615800802478247. PMC 2766521. PMID 19096959.
  88. ^ a b Cousens, G.A.; Kearns, A.; Laterza, F.; Tundidor, J. (2012). "Excitotoxic lesions of the medial amygdala attenuate olfactory fear-potentiated startle and conditioned freezing behavior". Behavioural Brain Research. 229 (2): 427–432. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.011. PMID 22249137. S2CID 25116098.
  89. ^ Jovanovic, T; Norrholm, Seth D.; Fennell, Jennifer E.; Keyes, Megan; Fiallos, Ana M.; Myers, Karyn M.; Davis, Michael; Duncan, Erica J. (15 May 2009). "Posttraumatic stress disorder may be associated with impaired fear inhibition: relation to symptom severity". Psychiatry Research. 167 (1–2): 151–160. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.12.014. PMC 2713500. PMID 19345420.
  90. ^ Picard, R. W.; Papert, S; Bender, W; Blumberg, B; Breazeal, C; Cavallo, D; Machover, T; Resnick, M; Roy, D; Strohecker, C (2004). "Affective Learning – a manifesto". BT Technology Journal. 22 (4): 253–269. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.110.3308. doi:10.1023/b:bttj.0000047603.37042.33. S2CID 518899. Closed access icon
  91. ^ Havas, D.A.; Glenberg, A.M.; Rinck, M. (2007). "Emotion simulation during language comprehension". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 14 (3): 436–441. doi:10.3758/bf03194085. PMID 17874584.
  92. ^ Wicker, B.; Keysers, Christian; Plailly, Jane; Royet, Jean-Pierre; Gallese, Vittorio; Rizzolatti, Giacomo (2003). "Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and Feeling Disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287. S2CID 766157.
  93. ^ Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). "Embodying emotion". Science. 316 (5827): 1002–1005. Bibcode:2007Sci...316.1002N. doi:10.1126/science.1136930. PMID 17510358. S2CID 14537829.
  94. ^ a b c Celeghin, Alessia; Diano, Matteo; Bagnis, Arianna; Viola, Marco; Tamietto, Marco (2017). "Basic Emotions in Human Neuroscience: Neuroimaging and Beyond". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1432. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01432. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5573709. PMID 28883803. This article incorporates text by Alessia Celeghin, Matteo Diano, Arianna Bagnis, Marco Viola, and Marco Tamietto available under the CC BY 4.0 license.
  95. ^ a b c Ekman, P.; Cordaro, D. (2011). "What is meant by calling emotions basic". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 364–370. doi:10.1177/1754073911410740. S2CID 52833124.
  96. ^ a b c Panksepp, J.; Watt, D. (2011). "What is basic about basic emotions? Lasting lessons from affective neuroscience". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 387–396. doi:10.1177/1754073911410741. S2CID 144198152.
  97. ^ Izard, C.E. (2011). "Forms and functions in emotions: Matters of emotion-cognition interactions". Emotion Review. 3 (4): 371–378. doi:10.1177/1754073911410737. S2CID 144037625.
  98. ^ Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.
  99. ^ a b c Barrett, L.F.; Wager, T. (2006). "The structure of emotion: Evidence from the neuroimaging of emotion". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (2): 79–85. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.470.7762. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00411.x. S2CID 14489624.
  100. ^ a b Lindquist, K.; Wager, T.; Kober, Hedy; Bliss-Moreau, Eliza; Barrett, Lisa Feldman (2012). "The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 35 (3): 121–143. doi:10.1017/s0140525x11000446. PMC 4329228. PMID 22617651.
  101. ^ Fox, Andrew S.; Shackman, Alexander J. (February 2019). "The central extended amygdala in fear and anxiety: Closing the gap between mechanistic and neuroimaging research". Neuroscience Letters. 693: 58–67. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2017.11.056. ISSN 0304-3940. PMC 5976525. PMID 29195911.
  102. ^ Phan, K.L.; Wager, T.D.; Taylor, S.F.; Liberzon, I. (2002). "Functional neuroanatomy of emotion: A meta-analysis of emotion activation studies in PET and fMRI". NeuroImage. 16 (2): 331–348. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1087. PMID 12030820. S2CID 7150871.
  103. ^ Murphy, F.C.; Nimmo-Smith, I.; Lawrence, A.D. (2003). "Functional Neuroanatomy: A meta-analysis". Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 3 (3): 207–233. doi:10.3758/cabn.3.3.207. PMID 14672157.
  104. ^ a b Wager, T.D.; Lindquist, M.; Kaplan, L. (2007). "Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging data: Current and future directions". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2 (2): 150–158. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm015. PMC 2555451. PMID 18985131.
  105. ^ Kober, H.; Barrett, L.F.; Joseph, J.; Bliss-Moreau, E.; Lindquist, K.; Wager, T.D. (2008). "Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies". NeuroImage. 42 (2): 998–1031. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.059. PMC 2752702. PMID 18579414.
  106. ^ Vytal, K.; Hamann, S. (2010). "Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (12): 2864–2885. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21366. PMID 19929758. S2CID 18151469.

Further reading

  • Davidson, R.J.; Irwin, W. (1999). "The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3 (1): 11–21. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(98)01265-0. PMID 10234222. S2CID 30912026.
  • Panksepp, J. (1992). "A critical role for affective neuroscience in resolving what is basic about basic emotions". Psychological Review. 99 (3): 554–60. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.99.3.554. PMID 1502276.
  • Harmon-Jones E, & Winkielman P. (Eds.) Social Neuroscience: Integrating Biological and Psychological Explanations of Social Behavior. New York: Guilford Publications.
  • Cacioppo, J.T., & Berntson, G.G. (2005). Social Neuroscience. Psychology Press.
  • Cacioppo, J.T., Tassinary, L.G., & Berntson, G.G. (2007). Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press.
  • Panksepp J. (1998). Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (Series in Affective Science). Oxford University Press, New York, New York.
  • Brain and Cognition, Vol. 52, No. 1, pp. 1–133 (June, 2003). Special Issue on Affective Neuroscience.