Biology and sexual orientation: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
Typo fixing, typos fixed: exibiting → exhibiting, et al → et al. using AWB
Steel (talk | contribs)
from http://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?diff=267607414
Line 120: Line 120:


Brendan Zietsch of the Queensland Institute of Medical Research proposes the alternative theory that men exhibiting female traits become more attractive to females and are thus more likely to mate, provided the genes involved to not drive them to complete rejection of heterosexuality.<ref>[http://www.economist.com/science/displaystory.cfm?story_id=12465295 How homosexuality may have evolved]</ref>
Brendan Zietsch of the Queensland Institute of Medical Research proposes the alternative theory that men exhibiting female traits become more attractive to females and are thus more likely to mate, provided the genes involved to not drive them to complete rejection of heterosexuality.<ref>[http://www.economist.com/science/displaystory.cfm?story_id=12465295 How homosexuality may have evolved]</ref>

==Biological differences in gay men and lesbians==
=== Physiological ===

Recent studies have found notable differences between the physiology of gay people and non-gay people. There is evidence that:
* Gay men have similar brains to those of straight women and gay women to those of straight men <ref>[http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/7456588.stm Scans see 'gay brain differences' - BBC News]</ref>
* The average size of the [[INAH 3|INAH-3]] in the brains of gay men is approximately the same size as INAH 3 in women, which is significantly smaller, and the cells more densely packed, than in heterosexual men's brains.<ref name="levay">{{cite journal |author=LeVay S |title=A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men |journal=Science |volume=253 |issue=5023 |pages=1034–7 |year=1991 |pmid=1887219 |doi=10.1126/science.1887219}}</ref>
* The [[suprachiasmatic nucleus]] was found by Swaab and Hopffman to be larger in gay men than in non-gay men,<ref>http://www.dafml.unito.it/anatomy/panzica/pubblicazioni/pdf/1995PanzicaJEI.pdf</ref> the suprachiasmatic nucleus is also known to be larger in men than in women.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Swaab DF, Zhou JN, Ehlhart T, Hofman MA |title=Development of vasoactive intestinal polypeptide neurons in the human suprachiasmatic nucleus in relation to birth and sex |journal=Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. |volume=79 |issue=2 |pages=249–59 |year=1994 |pmid=7955323 |doi=}}</ref>
* The [[anterior commissure]] is larger in women than men and was reported to be larger in gay men than in non-gay men,<ref>{{cite journal |author=Allen LS, Gorski RA |title=Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain |journal=Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. |volume=89 |issue=15 |pages=7199–202 |year=1992 |pmid=1496013 |doi=}}</ref> but a subsequent study found no such difference.<ref>Lasco, M. S., Jordan, T. J., Edgar, M. A., Petito, C. K., & Byne, W. (2002). A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation in the human anterior commissure. ''Brain Research, 936,'' 95–98.</ref>
* Gay men report, on an average, slightly longer and thicker penises than non-gay men.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Bogaert AF, Hershberger S |title=The relation between sexual orientation and penile size |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=28 |issue=3 |pages=213–21 |year=1999 |pmid=10410197 |doi=10.1023/A:1018780108597}}</ref>
* Gay men's brains respond differently to [[fluoxetine]], a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Kinnunen LH, Moltz H, Metz J, Cooper M |title=Differential brain activation in exclusively homosexual and heterosexual men produced by the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, fluoxetine |journal=Brain Res. |volume=1024 |issue=1-2 |pages=251–4 |year=2004 |pmid=15451388 |doi=10.1016/j.brainres.2004.07.070}}</ref>
* The functioning of the inner ear and the central auditory system in lesbians and bisexual women are more like the functional properties found in men than in non-gay women (the researchers argued this finding was consistent with the [[Prenatal hormones and sexual orientation|prenatal hormonal theory of sexual orientation]]).<ref name="mcf">{{cite journal |author=McFadden D |title=Masculinization effects in the auditory system |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=31 |issue=1 |pages=99–111 |year=2002 |pmid=11910797 |doi=10.1023/A:1014087319682}}</ref>
* The [[startle response]] (eyeblink following a loud sound) is similarly masculinized in lesbians and bisexual women.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Rahman Q, Kumari V, Wilson GD |title=Sexual orientation-related differences in prepulse inhibition of the human startle response |journal=Behav. Neurosci. |volume=117 |issue=5 |pages=1096–102 |year=2003 |pmid=14570558 |doi=10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.1096}}</ref>
* Three regions of the brain ([[medial prefrontal cortex]], left hippocampus, and right amygdala) are more active in gay men than non-gay men when exposed to sexually arousing material.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Safron A, Barch B, Bailey JM, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Reber PJ |title=Neural correlates of sexual arousal in homosexual and heterosexual men |journal=Behav. Neurosci. |volume=121 |issue=2 |pages=237–48 |year=2007 |pmid=17469913 |doi=10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.237}}</ref>
* Gay and non-gay people emit different armpit odors.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Martins Y, Preti G, Crabtree CR, Runyan T, Vainius AA, Wysocki CJ |title=Preference for human body odors is influenced by gender and sexual orientation |journal=Psychol Sci |volume=16 |issue=9 |pages=694–701 |year=2005 |pmid=16137255 |doi=10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01598.x}}</ref>
* Gay men are more likely to have a counter-clockwise hair whorl pattern.<ref>http://www.iisc.ernet.in/academy/jgenet/Vol83No3/251.pdf</ref>
* Gay and non-gay people's brains respond differently to two human sex pheromones (AND, found in male armpit secretions, and EST, found in female urine).<ref>{{cite journal |author=Savic I, Berglund H, Gulyas B, Roland P |title=Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans |journal=Neuron |volume=31 |issue=4 |pages=661–8 |year=2001 |pmid=11545724 |doi=10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00390-7}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Savic I, Berglund H, Lindström P |title=Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men |journal=Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. |volume=102 |issue=20 |pages=7356–61 |year=2005 |pmid=15883379 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0407998102}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Berglund H, Lindström P, Savic I |title=Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women |journal=Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. |volume=103 |issue=21 |pages=8269–74 |year=2006 |pmid=16705035 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0600331103}}</ref>
* [[Digit ratio|Finger length ratios]] between the index and ring fingers may be different between non-gay and lesbian women.<ref>{{cite journal |author=Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM |title=Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia |journal=Horm Behav |volume=42 |issue=4 |pages=380–6 |year=2002 |pmid=12488105 |doi=10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Hines M, Johnston KJ, Golombok S, Rust J, Stevens M, Golding J |title=Prenatal stress and gender role behavior in girls and boys: a longitudinal, population study |journal=Horm Behav |volume=42 |issue=2 |pages=126–34 |year=2002 |pmid=12367566 |doi=10.1006/hbeh.2002.1814}}</ref><ref name="mcf"/><ref>{{cite journal |author=Rahman Q, Wilson GD |title=Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability? |journal=Psychoneuroendocrinology |volume=28 |issue=3 |pages=288–303 |year=2003 |pmid=12573297 |doi=10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00022-7}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Brown WM, Finn CJ, Cooke BM, Breedlove SM |title=Differences in finger length ratios between self-identified "butch" and "femme" lesbians |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=31 |issue=1 |pages=123–7 |year=2002 |pmid=11910785 |doi=10.1023/A:1014091420590}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Hall LS, Love CT |title=Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discordant for sexual orientation |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=32 |issue=1 |pages=23–8 |year=2003 |pmid=12597269 |doi=10.1023/A:1021837211630}}</ref>

=== Cognitive ===

Recent studies suggest the presence of subtle differences in the way gay people and non-gay people process certain kinds of information. Researchers have found that:
* Gay men and lesbians are significantly [[Handedness and sexual orientation|more likely to be left-handed or ambidextrous]] than are non-gay men and women;<ref>{{cite journal |author=Lalumière ML, Blanchard R, Zucker KJ |title=Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis |journal=Psychol Bull |volume=126 |issue=4 |pages=575–92 |year=2000 |pmid=10900997 |doi=}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Mustanski BS, Bailey JM, Kaspar S |title=Dermatoglyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=31 |issue=1 |pages=113–22 |year=2002 |pmid=11910784 |doi=10.1023/A:1014039403752}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Lippa RA |title=Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related personality traits in men and women |journal=Arch Sex Behav |volume=32 |issue=2 |pages=103–14 |year=2003 |pmid=12710825 |doi=10.1023/A:1022444223812}}</ref> Simon LeVay argues that because "[h]and preference is observable before birth<ref>{{cite journal |author=Hepper PG, Shahidullah S, White R |title=Handedness in the human fetus |journal=Neuropsychologia |volume=29 |issue=11 |pages=1107–11 |year=1991 |pmid=1775228 |doi=10.1016/0028-3932(91)90080-R}}</ref>... [t]he observation of increased non-right-handness in gay people is therefore consistent with the idea that sexual orientation is influenced by prenatal processes," perhaps heredity.<ref name="levay"/>
* Gay men<ref>Geoff Sanders, Ph.D. and Marian Wright, B.Sc.(1997), Sexual Orientation Differences in Cerebral Asymmetry and in the Performance of Sexually Dimorphic Cognitive and Motor Tasks</ref> and lesbians are more verbally [[fluency|fluent]] than heterosexuals of the same sex<ref>[http://inductivist.blogspot.com/2007/09/homosexuality-and-iq-looking-at.html GSS data on verbal performance of homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual males and females]</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=McCormick CM, Witelson SF |title=A cognitive profile of homosexual men compared to heterosexual men and women |journal=Psychoneuroendocrinology |volume=16 |issue=6 |pages=459–73 |year=1991 |pmid=1811244 |doi=}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Rahman Q, Abrahams S, Wilson GD |title=Sexual-orientation-related differences in verbal fluency |journal=Neuropsychology |volume=17 |issue=2 |pages=240–6 |year=2003 |pmid=12803429 |doi=}}</ref> (but two studies did not find this result).<ref>Gladue, B. A., W. W. Beatty, et al. (1990). "Sexual orientation and spatial ability in men and women." Psychobiology 18: 101-108.</ref><ref>{{cite journal |author=Neave N, Menaged M, Weightman DR |title=Sex differences in cognition: the role of testosterone and sexual orientation |journal=Brain Cogn |volume=41 |issue=3 |pages=245–62 |year=1999 |pmid=10585237 |doi=10.1006/brcg.1999.1125}}</ref>
* Gay men may receive higher scores than non-gay men on tests of object location memory (no difference was found between lesbians and non-gay women).<ref>{{cite journal |author=Rahman Q, Wilson GD, Abrahams S |title=Sexual orientation related differences in spatial memory |journal=J Int Neuropsychol Soc |volume=9 |issue=3 |pages=376–83 |year=2003 |pmid=12666762 |doi=10.1017/S1355617703930037}}</ref>


== Political aspects ==
== Political aspects ==

Revision as of 12:27, 31 January 2009

Biology and sexual orientation is research into possible biological influences on the development of human sexual orientation. No simple cause for sexual orientation has been conclusively demonstrated, and there is no scientific consensus as to whether the contributing factors are primarily biological or environmental. Many think both play complex roles.[1][2] The American Academy of Pediatrics and the American Psychological Association have both stated that sexual orientation probably has multiple causes.[3][4] Research has identified several biological factors which may be related to the development of a heterosexual, homosexual or bisexual orientation. These include genes, prenatal hormones, and brain structure. Conclusive proof of a biological cause of sexual orientation would have significant political and cultural implications. [5]

Empirical studies

Twin studies

Researchers have traditionally used twin studies to try to isolate genetic influences from environmental or other influences. One common type of twin study compares identical twins (known as monozygotic or "MZ twins") who both have a particular trait to non-identical or fraternal twins (known as dizygotic or "DZ twins") with that same trait. Since identical twins have the same genetic makeup (genotype) while non-identical twins share only 50% of their genes, a difference between these types of twins provides evidence of a genetic component. For example, if a high percentage of identical twins both have red hair (while a low percentage of non-identical twins both have red hair), that suggests that red hair has a genetic basis. On the other hand, if identical twins share a characteristic just as often as fraternal twins (such as love of music), that suggests that there is not a genetic basis for that trait.

A number of twin studies have attempted this kind of isolation. As Bearman and Bruckner (2002)[6] describe it, early studies concentrated on small, select samples, which showed very high genetic influences; however, they were also criticized for non-representative selection of their subjects.[7] Later studies, performed on increasingly representative samples, showed much lesser concordance among MZ twins, although still significantly larger than among DZ twins.

For example, a recent meta-study by Hershberger (2001)[8] compares the results of eight different twin studies: among those, all but two showed MZ twins having much higher concordance of sexual orientation than DZ twins, suggesting a non-negligible genetic component. Two additional examples: Bailey and Pillard (1991) in a study of gay twins found that 52% of monozygotic (MZ) brothers and 22% of the dizygotic (DZ) twins were concordant for homosexuality.[9] Also, Bailey, Dunne and Martin (2000) used the Australian twin registry to obtain a sample of 4,901 twins.[10] Self reported zygosity, sexual attraction, fantasy and behaviours were assessed by questionnaire and zygosity was serologically checked when in doubt. MZ twin concordance for homosexuality was found to be 30%.

As a counter-example, Bearman and Bruckner (2002), analyzed data from a large longitudinal study of adolescents. They found the data did not support genetic influence:

Among [identical] twins, 6.7% are concordant [that is, both express same-sex romantic attraction]. [Fraternal] twin pairs are 7.2% concordant. Full-siblings are 5.5% concordant. Clearly, the observed concordance rates do not correspond to degrees of genetic similarity. None of the comparisons between [identical] twins and others ... are even remotely significant. If same-sex romantic attraction has a genetic component, it is massively overwhelmed by other factors.[6]

Their conclusion is that the expression of same-sex attraction requires a social environment: "More plausible is the idea that genetic expression is activated only under strongly circumscribed social structural conditions. In contrast to other theories considered below, we assume that the close connection between gender identity and sexual identity is socially constructed."

A recent study of all adult twins in Sweden (more than 7,600 twins)[11] found that same-sex behavior was explained by both heritable factors and individual-specific environmental sources (such as prenatal environment, experience with illness and trauma, as well as peer groups, and sexual experiences), while influences of shared-environment variables such as familial environment and societal attitudes had a weaker significant effect. Women showed a statistically non-significant trend to weaker influence of hereditary effects, while men showed no effect of shared environmental effects. The use of all adult twins in Sweden was designed to address the criticism of volunteer studies, in which a potential bias towards participation by gay twin may influence the results (see below).

Overall, the environment shared by twins (including familial and societal attitudes) explained 0-17% of the choice of sexual partner, genetic factors 18-39% and the unique environment 61-66%. The individual's unique environment includes, for example, circumstances during pregnancy and childbirth, physical and psychological trauma (e.g., accidents, violence, and disease), peer groups, and sexual experiences. [...] In men, genetic effects explained .34–.39 of the variance, the shared environment .00, and the individual-specific environment .61–.66 of the variance. Corresponding estimates among women were .18–.19 for genetic factors, .16–.17 for shared environmental, and 64–.66 for unique environmental factors.

Criticisms of Twin Studies

Twin studies have received a number of criticisms including self-selection bias where homosexuals with gay siblings are more likely to volunteer for studies. Nonetheless, it is possible to conclude that, given the difference in sexuality in so many sets of identical twins (who are genetically identical, and shared the same fetal environment), sexual orientation cannot be purely biologically caused.[12]

Another issue is the recent finding that even monozygotic twins can be different and there is a mechanism which might account for monozygotic twins being discordant for homosexuality. Gringas and Chen (2001) describe a number of mechanisms which can lead to differences between monozygotic twins, the most relevant here being chorionicity and amniocity.[13] Dichorionic twins potentially have different hormonal environments and receive maternal blood from separate placenta. Monoamniotic twins share a hormonal environment, but can suffer from the 'twin to twin transfusion syndrome' in which one twin is "relatively stuffed with blood and the other exsanguinated".[14] If one twin receives less testosterone and the other more, this could result in different levels of brain masculinisation.

Chromosome linkage studies

Earlier chromosome studies of homosexuality in males have not been replicated, or have had doubt cast on these early suggestions. For example, in 1993, Dean Hamer and colleagues published findings from a linkage analysis of a sample of 76 gay brothers and their families.[15] Hamer et al. found that the gay men had more gay male uncles and cousins on the maternal side of the family than on the paternal side. Gay brothers who showed this maternal pedigree were then tested for X chromosome linkage, using twenty-two markers on the X chromosome to test for similar alleles. In another finding, thirty-three of the forty sibling pairs tested were found to have similar alleles in the distal region of Xq28, which was significantly higher than the expected rates of 50% for fraternal brothers. This was popularly (but inaccurately) dubbed as the 'gay gene' in the media, causing significant controversy.

However, a later analysis by Hu et al. revealed that 67% of gay brothers in a new saturated sample shared a marker on the X chromosome at Xq28.[16] Sanders et al. (1998) replicated the study, finding 66% Xq28 marker sharing in 54 pairs of gay brothers.[17] On the other hand, two other studies (Bailey et al., 1999; McKnight and Malcolm, 2000) failed to find a preponderance of gay relatives in the maternal line of homosexual men.[17] Also, a study by Rice et al. in 1999 failed to replicate the Xq28 linkage results.[18]

Additionally, Mustanski et al. (2005) performed a full-genome scan (instead of just an X chromosome scan) on individuals and families previously reported on in Hamer et al. (1993) and Hu et al. (1995), as well as additional new subjects.[19] With the larger sample set and complete genome scan, the study found much weaker link for Xq28 than reported by Hamer et al. However, they did find other markers with significant likelihood scores at 8p12, 7q36 and 10q26, the latter two having approximately equivalent maternal and paternal contributions.

Epigenetics studies

A recent study suggests linkage between a mother's genetic make-up and homosexuality of her sons. Women have two X chromosomes, one of which is "switched off". The inactivation of the X chromosome occurs randomly throughout the embryo, resulting in cells that are mosaic with respect to which chromosome is active. In some cases though, it appears that this switching off can occur in a non-random fashion. Bocklandt et al. (2006) reported that, in mothers of homosexual men, the number of women with extreme skewing of X chromosome inactivation is significantly higher than in mothers without gay sons. Thirteen percent of mothers with one gay son, and 23% of mothers with two gay sons showed extreme skewing, compared to 4% percent of mothers without gay sons.[20] One problem in building consensus using this type of study stems from their heavy reliance on participants truthfully reporting their sexual preference.

Maternal linkage, birth order, and female fertility

Blanchard and Klassen (1997) reported that each older brother increases the odds of being gay by 33%.[21][22] This is now "one of the most reliable epidemiological variables ever identified in the study of sexual orientation."[23] To explain this finding, it has been proposed that male fetuses provoke a maternal immune reaction that becomes stronger with each successive male fetus. Male fetuses produce HY antigens which are "almost certainly involved in the sexual differentiation of vertebrates." It is this antigen which maternal H-Y antibodies are proposed to both react to and 'remember'. Successive male fetuses are then attacked by H-Y antibodies which somehow decrease the ability of H-Y antigens to perform their usual function in brain masculinisation.[21]

Bocklandt, Horvath, Vilain and Hamer (2006) reported that some mothers of gay babies have extreme skewing of X chromosome inactivation. Using a sample of 97 mothers of homosexual men and 103 mothers of heterosexual men, the pattern of X inactivation was ascertained from blood assays. 4% of the mothers of straight men showed extreme skewing compared to 13% of the mothers of gay men. Mothers of two or more gay babies had extreme skewing of X inactivation of 23%. This extreme skewing may influence male sexual orientation through the fraternal birth order effect.[20]

An alternate theory was proposed by Italian researchers in 2004 supported by a study of about 4,600 people who were the relatives of 98 homosexual and 100 heterosexual men. Female relatives of the homosexual men tended to have more offspring than those of the heterosexual men. Female relatives of the homosexual men on their mother's side tended to have more offspring than those on the father's side. The researchers concluded that there was genetic material being passed down on the X chromosome which both promotes fertility in the mother and homosexuality in her male offspring. The connections discovered, however, would explain only 20% of the cases studied, indicating that this might not be the sole genetic factor determining sexual orientation.[24]

Homosexuals of either sex are more likely than the general population to be non-right-handed.[25] (See also Handedness and sexual orientation.)

Pheromone studies

Recent research conducted in Sweden[26] has suggested that gay and straight men respond differently to two odors that are believed to be involved in sexual arousal. The research showed that when both heterosexual women (lesbians were included in the study, but the results regarding them were "somewhat confused") and gay men are exposed to a testosterone derivative found in men's sweat, a region in the hypothalamus is activated. Heterosexual men, on the other hand, have a similar response to an estrogen-like compound found in women's urine.[27] The conclusion, that sexual attraction, whether same-sex or opposite-sex oriented, operates similarly on a biological level, does not mean that there is necessarily a biological cause for homosexuality. Researchers have suggested that this possibility could be further explored by studying young subjects to see if similar responses in the hypothalamus are found and then correlating this data with adult sexual orientation.[citation needed]

Another form of research was done by Dr. Savic. Dr. Savic uses PET scans to see the brain activity while letting people smell different types of pheromones. Savic used two compounds that resemble the male and female sex hormones. The first is 4,16-androstadien-3-one (AND) which is a derivative of testosterone produced in human axillary secretions in higher concentrations in men than in women. The second compound is oestra-1,3,5(10),16-tetraen-3-ol (EST) which is a substance resembling naturally occurring oestrogenes [28]. These are the pheromones that make men and women attracted to each other. When a man smells a woman's pheromone, EST, there is a degree of brain activity. The same happens to women for the pheromone AND. Savic found that gays had the similar brain activity as women when given the whiff of AND, and vice versa for lesbians [29]. Savic's findings imply that sexual orientation is determined prior to exposure to life’s environmental influences. Also, unlike some of the early researchers, Savic's research is less likely to cater to a gay political agenda or bias, as her field was originally epilepsy research. She inadvertently stumbled onto the pheromone sex differences while studying how smells might trigger temporal lobe epilepsy [30].

Studies of brain structure

A number of sections of the brain have been reported to be sexually dimorphic; that is, they vary between men and women. There have also been reports of variations in brain structure corresponding to sexual orientation. In 1990, Swaab and Hofman reported a difference in the size of the suprachiasmatic nucleus between homosexual and heterosexual men.[31] In 1992, Allen and Gorski reported a difference related to sexual orientation in the size of the anterior commissure.[32]

Early work of this type was also done by Simon LeVay. LeVay studied four groups of neurons in the hypothalamus, called INAH1, INAH2, INAH3 and INAH4. This was a relevant area of the brain to study, because of evidence that this part of the brain played a role in the regulation of sexual behaviour in animals, and because INAH2 and INAH3 had previously been reported to differ in size between men and women.[33]

He obtained brains from 41 deceased hospital patients. The subjects were classified as follows: 19 gay men who had died of AIDS, 16 presumed heterosexual men (6 of whom had died of AIDS), and 6 presumed heterosexual women (1 of whom had died of AIDS).[33] The AIDS patients in the heterosexual groups were all identified from medical records as intravenous drug abusers or recipients of blood transfusions, though only 2 of the men in this category had specifically denied homosexual activity. The records of the remaining heterosexual subjects contained no information about their sexual orientation; they were assumed to have been mostly or all heterosexual "on the basis of the numerical preponderance of heterosexual men in the population."[33] LeVay found no evidence for a difference between the groups in the size of INAH1, INAH2 or INAH4. However, the INAH3 group appeared to be twice as big in the heterosexual male group as in the gay male group; the difference was highly significant, and remained significant when only the 6 AIDS patients were included in the heterosexual group. The size of the INAH3 in the homosexual male brains was similar to that in the heterosexual female brains. However, he also found some contrary results:

  • Three of the 19 homosexual subjects had a larger group of neurons in the hypothalamus than the average control-group subject.
  • Three of the 16 control-group subjects had a smaller group of neurons in the hypothalamus than the average homosexual subject.[33]
  • One of the subjects classified as gay was actually bisexual. This subject's INAH3 group was about the same size as the heterosexual group.[34]

William Byne and colleagues attempted to replicate the differences reported in INAH 1-4 size using a different sample of brains from 14 HIV-positive homosexual males, 34 presumed heterosexual males (10 HIV-positive), and 34 presumed heterosexual females (9 HIV-positive). They found a significant difference in INAH3 size between heterosexual men and women. The INAH3 size of the homosexual men was apparently smaller than that of the heterosexual men and larger than that of the heterosexual women, though neither difference quite reached statistical significance.[35]

Byne and colleagues also weighed and counted numbers of neurons in INAH3, tests not carried out by LeVay. The results for INAH3 weight were similar to those for INAH3 size; that is, the INAH3 weight for the heterosexual male brains was significantly larger than for the heterosexual female brains, while the results for the gay male group were between those of the other two groups but not quite significantly different from either. The neuron count also found a male-female difference in INAH3, but found no trend related to sexual orientation.[35]

Conclusions

LeVay concluded in his 1991 paper that "The discovery that the nucleus differs in size between heterosexual and homosexual men illustrates that sexual orientation in humans is amenable to study at the biological level, and this discovery opens the door to studies of neurotransmitters or receptors that might be involved in regulating this aspect of personality. Further interpretation of the results of this study must be considered speculative. In particular, the results do not allow one to decide if the size of INAH 3 in an individual is the cause or consequence of that individual's sexual orientation, or if the size of INAH 3 and sexual orientation covary under the influence of some third, unidentified variable."[33]

He later added,

"It's important to stress what I didn't find. I did not prove that homosexuality is genetic, or find a genetic cause for being gay. I didn't show that gay men are born that way, the most common mistake people make in interpreting my work. Nor did I locate a gay center in the brain. ... Since I look at adult brains, we don't know if the differences I found were there at birth or if they appeared later." [36]

Biological theories of etiology of sexual orientation

Early fixation hypothesis

The early fixation hypothesis includes research into prenatal development and the environmental factors that control masculinization of the brain. Studies have concluded that there is empirical evidence to support this hypothesis, including the observed differences in brain structure and cognitive processing between homosexual and heterosexual men. One explanation for these differences is the idea that differential exposure to hormone levels in the womb during fetal development may block or exaggerate masculinization of the brain in homosexual men. The concentrations of these chemicals is thought to be influenced by fetal and maternal immune systems, maternal consumption of certain drugs, maternal stress, and direct injection. This hypothesis is also connected to the fraternal birth order research.

Imprinting/critical period

This type of theory holds that the formation of gender identity occurs in the first few years of life after birth. It argues that individuals can be predisposed to homosexual orientation by biological factors but are triggered in some cases by upbringing. Part of adopting a gender identity involves establishing the gender(s) of sexual attraction. This process is analogous to the "imprinting" process observed in animals. A baby duckling may be genetically programmed to "imprint" on a mother, but what entity it actually imprints upon depends on what objects it sees immediately after hatching. Most importantly, once this process has occurred, it cannot be reversed, any more than the duckling can hatch twice.

A sort of reverse sexual imprinting has been observed in heterosexual humans; see the section on the "Westermarck effect" in Behavioral imprinting.

Several different triggers for imprinting upon a particular sexual orientation have been proposed.

One hypothesis is that something about what young children see in the gender-roles behavior of adults, or some differences (possibly unconscious) in the way adults treat young children, somehow influences or determines a child's eventual sexual orientation.

Exotic becomes erotic

Daryl Bem, a social psychologist at Cornell University, has theorized that the influence of biological factors on sexual orientation may be mediated by experiences in childhood. A child's temperament predisposes the child to prefer certain activities over others. Because of their temperament, which is influenced by biological variables such as genetic factors, some children will be attracted to activities that are commonly enjoyed by other children of the same gender. Others will prefer activities that are typical of another gender. This will make a gender-conforming child feel different from opposite-gender children, while gender-nonconforming children will feel different from children of their own gender. According to Bem, this feeling of difference will evoke physiological arousal when the child is near members of the gender which it considers as being 'different'. Bem theorizes that this physiological arousal will later be transformed into sexual arousal: children will become sexually attracted to the gender which they see as different ("exotic"). This theory is known as Exotic Becomes Erotic (EBE) theory.[37]

The theory is based in part on the frequent finding that a majority of gay men and lesbians report being gender-nonconforming during their childhood years. A meta-analysis of 48 studies showed childhood gender nonconformity to be the strongest predictor of a homosexual orientation for both men and women.[38] Fourteen studies published since Bailey & Zucker's 1995 also show the same results.[39] In one study by the Kinsey Institute of approximately 1000 gay men and lesbians (and a control group of 500 heterosexual men and women), 63% of both gay men and lesbians reported that they were gender nonconforming in childhood (i.e., did not like activities typical of their sex), compared with only 10-15% of heterosexual men and women. There are also six "prospective" studies--that is longitudinal studies that begin with gender-nonconforming boys at about age 7 and follow them up into adolescence and adulthood. These also show that a majority (63%) of the gender nonconforming boys become gay or bisexual as adults.[40] There are very few prospective studies of gender nonconforming girls.[41][42] In a group of eighteen behaviorally masculine girls (mean age of assessment: 9 years), all reported a homosexual sexual orientation at adolescence, and eight had requested sex reassignment.[43]

William Reiner, a psychiatrist and urologist with the University of Oklahoma has evaluated more than a hundred cases of children born with sexual differentiation disorders. In the 1960s and 70s, it was common in developed countries for doctors to castrate boys born with a micropenis and have them raised as girls. However, this practice has come under attack, because even though these boys were raised as girls, they nearly all report as adults that they are sexually attracted to women. This suggests that their sexual orientation was determined at birth. The only cases Reiner found where children born with a X and Y chromosome are attracted to males as adults were those where testosterone receptors were absent, which prevented the male sex hormones from masculinizing the fetus.

Pathogenic hypothesis of homosexuality

The pathogenic hypothesis of homosexuality, also called the 'gay germ' hypothesis, suggests that homosexuality might be caused by an infectious agent. The speculative hypothesis was suggested by Gregory Cochran and Paul Ewald as part of a larger project advocating a number of pathogenic theories of disease. They argue that because of the supposedly reduced number of offspring produced by gay and lesbian people, evolution would strongly select against it. They also draw an analogy to diseases that alter brain structure and behavior, such as narcolepsy, which are suspected of being triggered by viral infection.[44] Cochran also argues that the prevalence of homosexuality in urban areas suggests that an infectious disease causes homosexuality.[45] They conclude that it is a "feasible hypothesis... no more and no less."[44] After being unable to publish this account in a peer-reviewed journal, the idea appeared in the popular press.[46] An American Philosophical Association newsletter the following year stated "there is ultimately very little to be said in favor of these contentions", and criticised the press attention gained, given a lack of peer reviewed publication of the theory, and questioned the general ethics of communication of theories about homosexuality by researchers to the public. [47] In an article in Out Magazine, brain researcher William Byne stated "Cochran and Ewald are guilty of pathologizing homosexuality"[48], while in the same article psychology professor J. Michael Bailey posited that a 'germ theory' did not necessarily mean homosexuality was a disease, but recognised the political ammunition such a belief could give to homophobes. [48]

Sexual orientation and evolution

Sexual practices that significantly reduce the frequency of heterosexual intercourse also significantly decrease the chances of successful reproduction, and for this reason, they would appear to be maladaptive in an evolutionary context following a simple Darwinian model of Natural Selection—on the assumption that homosexuality would reduce this frequency.

Those who believe that homosexuality is purely genetic argue that maladaptive traits will only be removed from a population if the trait is under simple, direct selection, if it derives from a heritable component of a genotype and if the intensity of selection is greater than other evolutionary forces like genetic drift, or inclusive fitness.[citation needed]

Some scholars have suggested that homosexuality is adaptive in a non-obvious way. By way of analogy, the allele (a particular version of a gene) which causes sickle-cell anemia when two copies are present may also confer resistance to malaria with a lesser form of anemia when one copy is present (this is called heterozygous advantage).[citation needed]

The so-called "gay uncle" theory posits that people who themselves do not have children may nonetheless increase the prevalence of their family's genes in future generations by providing resources (food, supervision, defense, shelter, etc.) to the offspring of their closest relatives. This hypothesis is an extension of the theory of kin selection. Kin selection was originally developed to explain apparent altruistic acts which seemed to be maladaptive. The initial concept was suggested by J.B.S. Haldane in 1932 and later elaborated by many others including John Maynard Smith and West Eberhard.[49] This concept was also used to explain the patterns of certain social insects where most of the members are non-reproductive.

The primary criticism of this theory has to do with the fact that children share on average 25% of their genes with their uncles and aunts, but on average 50% with their parents. This means that to be adaptive, a "gay uncle" would need to somehow assist an extra two nieces or nephews, on average, to reach adulthood for every one of their own offspring they give up. Critics of the theory find this trade-off to be unlikely to produce a net reproductive gain.

Brendan Zietsch of the Queensland Institute of Medical Research proposes the alternative theory that men exhibiting female traits become more attractive to females and are thus more likely to mate, provided the genes involved to not drive them to complete rejection of heterosexuality.[50]

Biological differences in gay men and lesbians

Physiological

Recent studies have found notable differences between the physiology of gay people and non-gay people. There is evidence that:

  • Gay men have similar brains to those of straight women and gay women to those of straight men [51]
  • The average size of the INAH-3 in the brains of gay men is approximately the same size as INAH 3 in women, which is significantly smaller, and the cells more densely packed, than in heterosexual men's brains.[5]
  • The suprachiasmatic nucleus was found by Swaab and Hopffman to be larger in gay men than in non-gay men,[52] the suprachiasmatic nucleus is also known to be larger in men than in women.[53]
  • The anterior commissure is larger in women than men and was reported to be larger in gay men than in non-gay men,[54] but a subsequent study found no such difference.[55]
  • Gay men report, on an average, slightly longer and thicker penises than non-gay men.[56]
  • Gay men's brains respond differently to fluoxetine, a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor.[57]
  • The functioning of the inner ear and the central auditory system in lesbians and bisexual women are more like the functional properties found in men than in non-gay women (the researchers argued this finding was consistent with the prenatal hormonal theory of sexual orientation).[58]
  • The startle response (eyeblink following a loud sound) is similarly masculinized in lesbians and bisexual women.[59]
  • Three regions of the brain (medial prefrontal cortex, left hippocampus, and right amygdala) are more active in gay men than non-gay men when exposed to sexually arousing material.[60]
  • Gay and non-gay people emit different armpit odors.[61]
  • Gay men are more likely to have a counter-clockwise hair whorl pattern.[62]
  • Gay and non-gay people's brains respond differently to two human sex pheromones (AND, found in male armpit secretions, and EST, found in female urine).[63][64][65]
  • Finger length ratios between the index and ring fingers may be different between non-gay and lesbian women.[66][67][58][68][69][70]

Cognitive

Recent studies suggest the presence of subtle differences in the way gay people and non-gay people process certain kinds of information. Researchers have found that:

  • Gay men and lesbians are significantly more likely to be left-handed or ambidextrous than are non-gay men and women;[71][72][73] Simon LeVay argues that because "[h]and preference is observable before birth[74]... [t]he observation of increased non-right-handness in gay people is therefore consistent with the idea that sexual orientation is influenced by prenatal processes," perhaps heredity.[5]
  • Gay men[75] and lesbians are more verbally fluent than heterosexuals of the same sex[76][77][78] (but two studies did not find this result).[79][80]
  • Gay men may receive higher scores than non-gay men on tests of object location memory (no difference was found between lesbians and non-gay women).[81]

Political aspects

Whether genetic or other physiological determinants as the basis of sexual orientation is a highly politicised issue. The Advocate, a U.S. gay and lesbian newsmagazine, reported in 1996 that 61% of its readers believed that "it would mostly help gay and lesbian rights if homosexuality were found to be biologically determined".[82] A cross-national study in the United States, the Philippines, and Sweden found that those who believed that "homosexuals are born that way" held significantly more positive attitudes toward homosexuality than those who believed that "homosexuals choose to be that way" and/or "learn to be that way".[83][84]

The perceived causes of sexual orientation have a significant bearing on the status of sexual minorities such as lesbians and gays. The Family Research Council, a conservative Christian think tank in Washington, D.C., argues in the book Getting It Straight that finding people are born gay "would advance the idea that sexual orientation is an innate characteristic, like race; that homosexuals, like African-Americans, should be legally protected against 'discrimination;' and that disapproval of homosexuality should be as socially stigmatized as racism. However, it is not true."

Some advocates[who?] for the rights of sexual minorities also resist the idea that sexuality is biologically determined, or fixed at birth. They point out that sexual orientation can shift over the course of one's life. Many resist any attempts to pathologise or medicalise 'deviant' sexuality, and choose to fight for acceptance in a moral or social realm. Others[who?] fear that discoveries about medical causes of sexuality may be used by doctors and parents to eradicate non-heterosexual orientations.

See also

References

  1. ^ American Psychological Association Answers to Your Questions For a Better Understanding of Sexual Orientation & Homosexuality -

    There is no consensus among scientists about the exact reasons that an individual develops a heterosexual, bisexual, gay, or lesbian orientation. Although much research has examined the possible genetic, hormonal, developmental, social, and cultural influences on sexual orientation, no findings have emerged that permit scientists to conclude that sexual orientation is determined by any particular factor or factors. Many think that nature and nurture both play complex roles; most people experience little or no sense of choice about their sexual orientation.

  2. ^ "Gay, Lesbian and Bisexual Issues". Association of Gay and Lesbian Psychiatrics. 2000. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)

    No one knows what causes heterosexuality, homosexuality, or bisexuality.... there is a renewed interest in searching for biological etiologies for homosexuality. However, to date there are no replicated scientific studies supporting any specific biological etiology for homosexuality.

  3. ^ "Sexual Orientation and Adolescents" (PDF), American Academy of Pediatrics Clinical Report, retrieved 2007-02-23
  4. ^ "Answers to Your Questions About Sexual Orientation and Homosexuality", American Psychological Association, retrieved 2007-05-04
  5. ^ a b c LeVay, Simon (1996). Queer Science: The Use and Abuse of Research into Homosexuality. Cambridge: The MIT Press ISBN 0-262-12199-9 Cite error: The named reference "levay" was defined multiple times with different content (see the help page).
  6. ^ a b This work was published in the American Journal of Sociology (Bearman, P. S. & Bruckner, H. (2002) Opposite-sex twins and adolescent same-sex attraction. American Journal of Sociology 107, 1179–1205.) and is available only to subscribers. However, a final draft of the paper is available here - there are no significant differences on the points cited between the final draft and the published version.
  7. ^ While inconsistent with modern findings, the first relatively large-scale twin study on sexual orientation was reported by Kallman in 1952. (See: Kallmann FJ (1952). "Comparative twin study on the genetic aspects of male homosexuality". J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 115 (4): 283–97. PMID 14918012. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)). Examining only male twin pairs, he found a 100% concordance rate for homosexuality among 37 monozygotic (MZ) twin pairs, compared to a 12%-42% concordance rate among 26 dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs, depending on definition. In other words, every identical twin of a homosexual subject was also homosexual, while this was not the case for non-identical twins. This study was criticised for its vaguely described method of recruiting twins and for a high rate of psychiatric disorders among its subjects. (See Rosenthal, D., "Genetic Theory and Abnormal Behavior" 1970, New York: McGraw-Hill.)
  8. ^ Hershberger, Scott L. 2001. Biological Factors in the Development of Sexual Orientation. Pp. 27-51 in Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities and Youth: Psychological Perspectives, edited by Anthony R. D’Augelli and Charlotte J. Patterson. Oxford, New York: Oxford University Press. Quoted in Bearman and Bruckner, 2002.
  9. ^ cited in Wilson and Rahman 2005, p47
  10. ^ Bailey JM, Dunne MP, Martin NG (2000). "Genetic and environmental influences on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin sample". J Pers Soc Psychol. 78 (3): 524–36. PMID 10743878. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  11. ^ Långström, Niklas (7 June 2008). "Genetic and Environmental Effects on Same-sex Sexual Behaviour: A Population Study of Twins in Sweden". Archives of Sexual Behaviour. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9386-1. {{cite journal}}: Check date values in: |date= (help); Cite journal requires |journal= (help); Unknown parameter |coauthor= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  12. ^ Schacter, Daniel L., Gilbert, Daniel T., and Wegner, Daniel M. (2009) "Psychology". Worth Publishers: 435.
  13. ^ Gringas, P. and Chen, W. (2001). Mechanisms for difference in monozygous twins. Early Human Development, 64, (2), 105-117.
  14. ^ Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and Behavior. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.
  15. ^ Hamer DH, Hu S, Magnuson VL, Hu N, Pattatucci AM (1993). "A linkage between DNA markers on the X chromosome and male sexual orientation". Science (journal). 261 (5119): 321–7. doi:10.1126/science.8332896. PMID 8332896. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  16. ^ Hu S, Pattatucci AM, Patterson C; et al. (1995). "Linkage between sexual orientation and chromosome Xq28 in males but not in females". Nat. Genet. 11 (3): 248–56. doi:10.1038/ng1195-248. PMID 7581447. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  17. ^ a b Wilson, G.D., & Rahman, Q. (2005). Born Gay: The Biology of Sex Orientation. London: Peter Owen Publishers.
  18. ^ Vilain E (2000). "Genetics of sexual development". Annu Rev Sex Res. 11: 1–25. PMID 11351829.
  19. ^ Mustanski BS, Dupree MG, Nievergelt CM, Bocklandt S, Schork NJ, Hamer DH (2005). "A genomewide scan of male sexual orientation" (PDF). Hum. Genet. 116 (4): 272–8. doi:10.1007/s00439-004-1241-4. PMID 15645181. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  20. ^ a b Bocklandt S, Horvath S, Vilain E, Hamer DH (2006). "Extreme skewing of X chromosome inactivation in mothers of homosexual men". Hum. Genet. 118 (6): 691–4. doi:10.1007/s00439-005-0119-4. PMID 16369763. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  21. ^ a b Blanchard R, Klassen P (1997). "H-Y antigen and homosexuality in men". J. Theor. Biol. 185 (3): 373–8. doi:10.1006/jtbi.1996.0315. PMID 9156085. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  22. ^ Pas de Deux of Sexuality Is Written in the Genes
  23. ^ Blanchard R (1997). "Birth order and sibling sex ratio in homosexual versus heterosexual males and females". Annu Rev Sex Res. 8: 27–67. PMID 10051890.
  24. ^ Camperio-Ciani et al. 2004
  25. ^ Lalumière ML, Blanchard R, Zucker KJ (2000). "Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis". Psychol Bull. 126 (4): 575–92. PMID 10900997. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. ^ Savic I, Berglund H, Lindström P (2005). "Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (20): 7356–61. doi:10.1073/pnas.0407998102. PMC 1129091. PMID 15883379. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  27. ^ Wade, Nicholas. (May 9, 2005). "Gay Men Are Found to Have Different Scent of Attraction." New York Times.
  28. ^ “I Savic.” Molecular Psychiatry. 2003. 7 November 2008. <http://www.nature.com/mp/journal/v7/n4/full/4001094a.html>.
  29. ^ “Robert Burton.” Salon.com. 5 October 2008. 28 September 2008. <http://www.salon.com/env/mind_reader/2008/09/12/gay_neurology/index1.html>.
  30. ^ “Robert Burton.” Salon.com. 5 October 2008. 28 September 2008. <http://www.salon.com/env/mind_reader/2008/09/12/gay_neurology/index1.html>.
  31. ^ Swaab DF, Hofman MA (1990). "An enlarged suprachiasmatic nucleus in homosexual men". Brain Res. 537 (1–2): 141–8. PMID 2085769. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  32. ^ Allen LS, Gorski RA (1992). "Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89 (15): 7199–202. PMC 49673. PMID 1496013. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  33. ^ a b c d e LeVay S (1991). "A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men" (PDF). Science (journal). 253 (5023): 1034–7. doi:10.1126/science.1887219. PMID 1887219. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  34. ^ Van Wyk PH, Geist CS (1995). "Biology of bisexuality: critique and observations". J Homosex. 28 (3–4): 357–73. doi:10.1300/J082v28n03_11. PMID 7560936.
  35. ^ a b Byne W, Tobet S, Mattiace LA; et al. (2001). "The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypothalamus: an investigation of variation with sex, sexual orientation, and HIV status". Horm Behav. 40 (2): 86–92. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1680. PMID 11534967. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help); Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  36. ^ Sex and the Brain | Sex & the Brain | DISCOVER Magazine
  37. ^ Bem DJ, Herdt G, McClintock M (2000). "Exotic becomes erotic: interpreting the biological correlates of sexual orientation" (PDF). Arch Sex Behav. 29 (6): 531–48. PMID 11100261. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) PDF
  38. ^ Bailey, J.M., and Zucker, K.J. (1995). Childhood sex-typed behavior and sexual orientation: A conceptual analysis and quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 31(1): 43-55.
  39. ^ Zucker, K.J. (2005) Commentary on Gottschalk’s (2003) ‘Same-sex sexuality and childhood gender non-conformity: A spurious connection’ Journal of Gender Studies, 14:55–60.
  40. ^ Zucker, K.J. (1990) Gender identity disorders in children: clinical descriptions and natural history. p.1-23 in R. Blanchard & B.W. Steiner (eds) Clinical management of gender identity disorders in children and adults. Washington DC, American Psychiatric Press.
  41. ^ Green R (1979). "Childhood cross-gender behavior and subsequent sexual preference". Am J Psychiatry. 136 (1): 106–8. PMID 758811. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  42. ^ Cohen-Kettenis PT (2001). "Gender identity disorder in DSM?". J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 40 (4): 391. PMID 11314563. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)
  43. ^ Cohen-Kettenis, P. T. (2001) Gender identity disorder in DSM? [Letter to the editor], Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, p. 391. and comments reported in: Zucker, K.J. (2005) Commentary on Gottschalk’s (2003) ‘Same-sex sexuality and childhood gender non-conformity: A spurious connection’ Journal of Gender Studies, 14:55–60.
  44. ^ a b Crain, C. "Did a Germ Make You Gay?" in Out Magazine, August 1999.
  45. ^ An Evolutionary Look at Human Homosexuality Greg Cochran, original publication date unknown
  46. ^ http://www.apaonline.org/apa/publications/newsletters/v00n1/lgbt/04.asp
  47. ^ http://www.apaonline.org/apa/publications/newsletters/v00n1/lgbt/04.asp -

    Despite some degree of logical plausibility, there is ultimately very little to be said in favor of these contentions. In its focus on the reduced reproductive rates of homosexual men and women, the account ignores other mechanisms by which genetic traits endure across generations. More importantly, the account is offered without any evidence whatsoever about which microbe might work how to generate homosexual interests. A peer-reviewed science journal turned this account away, but it nevertheless found its way into the pages of the public press.... the ease with which theories of homosexuality seep into public discourse raises important ethical questions about the way in which researchers ought to communicate their various theories to the public. Given that an unfounded theory of homosexuality can do more damage than good, researchers should raise the bar in regard to the views they propound about its origin.

  48. ^ a b Crain, C. "Did a Germ Make You Gay?" in Out Magazine, August 1999 :

    On the one hand, William Byne, a brain researcher at the Mount Sinai School of Medicine in New York, suspects that Cochran and Ewald are guilty of pathologizing homosexuality. "It's hard for most people to entertain the idea that homosexuality might be a natural variant of human sexual behavior," says Byne. On the other hand, Michael Bailey, a professor of psychology at Northwestern University, gives Cochran and Ewald the benefit of the doubt. Bailey does worry that homophobes could use the germ theory as political ammunition—as "proof" that homosexuality is a disease. But that would be "a totally illegitimate conclusion," in Bailey's opinion. Not everything caused by a germ is a disease, he insists. "Suppose we found that a form of genius was also caused by a virus. Would that mean that genius is a disease?"

  49. ^ Mayr, E. (1982). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. p598.
  50. ^ How homosexuality may have evolved
  51. ^ Scans see 'gay brain differences' - BBC News
  52. ^ http://www.dafml.unito.it/anatomy/panzica/pubblicazioni/pdf/1995PanzicaJEI.pdf
  53. ^ Swaab DF, Zhou JN, Ehlhart T, Hofman MA (1994). "Development of vasoactive intestinal polypeptide neurons in the human suprachiasmatic nucleus in relation to birth and sex". Brain Res. Dev. Brain Res. 79 (2): 249–59. PMID 7955323.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  54. ^ Allen LS, Gorski RA (1992). "Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 89 (15): 7199–202. PMID 1496013.
  55. ^ Lasco, M. S., Jordan, T. J., Edgar, M. A., Petito, C. K., & Byne, W. (2002). A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation in the human anterior commissure. Brain Research, 936, 95–98.
  56. ^ Bogaert AF, Hershberger S (1999). "The relation between sexual orientation and penile size". Arch Sex Behav. 28 (3): 213–21. doi:10.1023/A:1018780108597. PMID 10410197.
  57. ^ Kinnunen LH, Moltz H, Metz J, Cooper M (2004). "Differential brain activation in exclusively homosexual and heterosexual men produced by the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, fluoxetine". Brain Res. 1024 (1–2): 251–4. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2004.07.070. PMID 15451388.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  58. ^ a b McFadden D (2002). "Masculinization effects in the auditory system". Arch Sex Behav. 31 (1): 99–111. doi:10.1023/A:1014087319682. PMID 11910797.
  59. ^ Rahman Q, Kumari V, Wilson GD (2003). "Sexual orientation-related differences in prepulse inhibition of the human startle response". Behav. Neurosci. 117 (5): 1096–102. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.1096. PMID 14570558.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  60. ^ Safron A, Barch B, Bailey JM, Gitelman DR, Parrish TB, Reber PJ (2007). "Neural correlates of sexual arousal in homosexual and heterosexual men". Behav. Neurosci. 121 (2): 237–48. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.237. PMID 17469913.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  61. ^ Martins Y, Preti G, Crabtree CR, Runyan T, Vainius AA, Wysocki CJ (2005). "Preference for human body odors is influenced by gender and sexual orientation". Psychol Sci. 16 (9): 694–701. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01598.x. PMID 16137255.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  62. ^ http://www.iisc.ernet.in/academy/jgenet/Vol83No3/251.pdf
  63. ^ Savic I, Berglund H, Gulyas B, Roland P (2001). "Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans". Neuron. 31 (4): 661–8. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00390-7. PMID 11545724.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  64. ^ Savic I, Berglund H, Lindström P (2005). "Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 (20): 7356–61. doi:10.1073/pnas.0407998102. PMID 15883379.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  65. ^ Berglund H, Lindström P, Savic I (2006). "Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103 (21): 8269–74. doi:10.1073/pnas.0600331103. PMID 16705035.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  66. ^ Brown WM, Hines M, Fane BA, Breedlove SM (2002). "Masculinized finger length patterns in human males and females with congenital adrenal hyperplasia". Horm Behav. 42 (4): 380–6. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1830. PMID 12488105.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  67. ^ Hines M, Johnston KJ, Golombok S, Rust J, Stevens M, Golding J (2002). "Prenatal stress and gender role behavior in girls and boys: a longitudinal, population study". Horm Behav. 42 (2): 126–34. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2002.1814. PMID 12367566.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  68. ^ Rahman Q, Wilson GD (2003). "Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability?". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 28 (3): 288–303. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00022-7. PMID 12573297.
  69. ^ Brown WM, Finn CJ, Cooke BM, Breedlove SM (2002). "Differences in finger length ratios between self-identified "butch" and "femme" lesbians". Arch Sex Behav. 31 (1): 123–7. doi:10.1023/A:1014091420590. PMID 11910785.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  70. ^ Hall LS, Love CT (2003). "Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discordant for sexual orientation". Arch Sex Behav. 32 (1): 23–8. doi:10.1023/A:1021837211630. PMID 12597269.
  71. ^ Lalumière ML, Blanchard R, Zucker KJ (2000). "Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis". Psychol Bull. 126 (4): 575–92. PMID 10900997.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  72. ^ Mustanski BS, Bailey JM, Kaspar S (2002). "Dermatoglyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation". Arch Sex Behav. 31 (1): 113–22. doi:10.1023/A:1014039403752. PMID 11910784.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  73. ^ Lippa RA (2003). "Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related personality traits in men and women". Arch Sex Behav. 32 (2): 103–14. doi:10.1023/A:1022444223812. PMID 12710825.
  74. ^ Hepper PG, Shahidullah S, White R (1991). "Handedness in the human fetus". Neuropsychologia. 29 (11): 1107–11. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90080-R. PMID 1775228.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  75. ^ Geoff Sanders, Ph.D. and Marian Wright, B.Sc.(1997), Sexual Orientation Differences in Cerebral Asymmetry and in the Performance of Sexually Dimorphic Cognitive and Motor Tasks
  76. ^ GSS data on verbal performance of homosexual, heterosexual, and bisexual males and females
  77. ^ McCormick CM, Witelson SF (1991). "A cognitive profile of homosexual men compared to heterosexual men and women". Psychoneuroendocrinology. 16 (6): 459–73. PMID 1811244.
  78. ^ Rahman Q, Abrahams S, Wilson GD (2003). "Sexual-orientation-related differences in verbal fluency". Neuropsychology. 17 (2): 240–6. PMID 12803429.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  79. ^ Gladue, B. A., W. W. Beatty, et al. (1990). "Sexual orientation and spatial ability in men and women." Psychobiology 18: 101-108.
  80. ^ Neave N, Menaged M, Weightman DR (1999). "Sex differences in cognition: the role of testosterone and sexual orientation". Brain Cogn. 41 (3): 245–62. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1125. PMID 10585237.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  81. ^ Rahman Q, Wilson GD, Abrahams S (2003). "Sexual orientation related differences in spatial memory". J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 9 (3): 376–83. doi:10.1017/S1355617703930037. PMID 12666762.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  82. ^ The Advocate (1996, February 6). Advocate Poll Results. p. 8.
  83. ^ Ernulf KE, Innala SM, Whitam FL (1989). "Biological explanation, psychological explanation, and tolerance of homosexuals: a cross-national analysis of beliefs and attitudes". Psychol Rep. 65 (3 Pt 1): 1003–10. PMID 2608821. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  84. ^ Whitley, B. E., Jr. (1990). The relationship of heterosexuals' attributions for the causes of homosexuality to attitudes toward lesbians and gay men. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 369-377.

Bibliography