Emotion: Difference between revisions
←Replaced content with '{{other uses}} {{redirect|Emotional}} {{Emotion}} In psychology and philosophy, '''emotion''' is sexual attraction' Tag: blanking |
AndyTheGrump (talk | contribs) Undid revision 643093949 by 185.5.154.248 (talk) rvv |
||
Line 2: | Line 2: | ||
{{redirect|Emotional}} |
{{redirect|Emotional}} |
||
{{Emotion}} |
{{Emotion}} |
||
In [[psychology]] and [[philosophy]], '''emotion''' is a [[subjectivity|subjective]], [[conscious]] [[subjective experience|experience]] characterized primarily by [[psychophysiology|psychophysiological]] [[Emotional expression|expressions]], [[metabolism|biological reactions]], and [[Mental state (disambiguation)|mental states]]. A similar multicomponential description of emotion is found in [[sociology]]. For example, Peggy Thoits<ref name="Thoits, P. A. 1989">Thoits, P. A. (1989). The sociology of emotions. Annual Review of Sociology, 15, 317-342.</ref> described emotions as involving physiological components, cultural or emotional labels (e.g., anger, surprise etc.), expressive body actions, and the appraisal of situations and contexts. Emotion is often associated and considered [[Reciprocal influence|reciprocally influential]] with [[Mood (psychology)|mood]], [[temperament]], [[Personality psychology|personality]], [[disposition]], and [[motivation]].<ref>{{cite web|url=http://psychology.about.com/od/psychologytopics/a/theories-of-emotion.htm |title=Theories of Emotion |publisher=Psychology.about.com |date=2013-09-13 |accessdate=2013-11-11}}</ref> It also is influenced by [[hormone]]s and [[neurotransmitter]]s such as [[dopamine]], [[noradrenaline]], [[serotonin]], [[oxytocin]], [[cortisol]] and [[Gamma-Aminobutyric acid|GABA]]. Emotion is often the driving force behind [[motivation]], positive or negative.<ref name = "Gaulin 6">Gaulin, Steven J. C. and Donald H. McBurney. Evolutionary Psychology. Prentice Hall. 2003. ISBN 978-0-13-111529-3, Chapter 6, p 121-142.</ref> An alternative definition of emotion is a "positive or negative experience that is associated with a particular pattern of physiological activity."<ref>{{cite book|last=Schacter|first=Daniel L.|title=Psychology Second Edition|year=2011|publisher=Worth Publishers|location=41 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10010|isbn=978-1-4292-3719-2|page=310}}</ref> |
|||
In [[psychology]] and [[philosophy]], '''emotion''' is sexual attraction |
|||
The [[physiology]] of emotion is closely linked to [[arousal]] of the [[nervous system]] with various states and strengths of arousal relating, apparently, to particular emotions. Emotions are a complex state of feeling that results in physical and psychological changes that influence our behaviour. Those acting primarily on emotion may seem as if they are not thinking, but [[cognition]] is an important aspect of emotion, particularly the interpretation of events. For example, the experience of fear usually occurs in response to a threat. The cognition of danger and subsequent arousal of the nervous system (e.g. rapid heartbeat and breathing, sweating, muscle tension) is an integral component to the subsequent interpretation and labeling of that arousal as an emotional state. Emotion is also linked to behavioral tendency. Extroverted people are more likely to be social and express their emotions, while introverted people are more likely to be more socially withdrawn and conceal their emotions. |
|||
Research on emotion has increased significantly over the past two decades with many fields contributing including [[psychology]], [[neuroscience]], [[endocrinology]], [[medicine]], [[history]], [[sociology]], and even [[computer science]]. The numerous theories that attempt to explain the origin, neurobiology, experience, and function of emotions have only fostered more intense research on this topic. Current areas of research in the concept of emotion include the development of materials that stimulate and elicit emotion. In addition [[PET scans]] and [[fMRI]] scans help study the affective processes in the brain.<ref>Cacioppo, J.T & Gardner, W.L (1999). Emotion. "Annual Review of Psychology", 191.</ref> |
|||
{{TOC limit|3}} |
|||
==Etymology, definitions, and differentiation== |
|||
The word "emotion" dates back to 1579, when it was adapted from the French word ''émouvoir'', which means "to stir up". However, the earliest precursors of the word likely dates back to the very origins of language.<ref>{{cite book|last=Merriam-Webster|title=The Merriam-Webster dictionary (11th ed.)|year=2004|publisher=Author|location=Springfield, MA}}</ref> Emotions are intense feelings that are directed at someone or something.<ref name="ReferenceA">Hume, D. Emotions and Moods. Organizational Behavior, 258-297.</ref> |
|||
Emotions have been described as discrete and consistent responses to internal or external events which have a particular significance for the organism. Emotions are brief in duration and consist of a coordinated set of responses, which may include verbal, [[physiological]], [[behavioural]], and [[neural]] mechanisms.{{sfn|Fox|2008|pp=16–17}} Psychotherapist Michael C. Graham describes all emotions as existing on a continuum of intensity.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Graham|first1=Michael C.|title=Facts of Life: ten issues of contentment|date=2014|publisher=Outskirts Press|pages=63|ISBN=978-1-4787-2259-5}}</ref> Thus fear might range from mild concern to terror or shame might range from simple embarrassment to toxic shame.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Graham|first1=Michael C.|title=Facts of Life: Ten Issues of Contentment|date=2014|publisher=Outskirts Press|ISBN=978-1-4787-2259-5}}</ref> Emotions have also been described as biologically given and a result of [[evolution]] because they provided good solutions to ancient and recurring problems that faced our ancestors.<ref>{{cite journal |last=Ekman |first=Paul |title=An argument for basic emotions |journal=Cognition & Emotion |year=1992 |volume=6 |pages=169–200 |doi=10.1080/02699939208411068}}</ref> Moods are feelings that tend to be less intense than emotions and that often lack a contextual stimulus.<ref name="ReferenceA"/> |
|||
Emotion can be differentiated from a number of similar constructs within the field of [[affective neuroscience]]:{{sfn|Fox|2008|pp=16–17}} |
|||
* '''[[Feeling]]s''' are best understood as a [[subjectivity|subjective]] representation of emotions, private to the individual experiencing them. |
|||
* '''[[Mood (psychology)|Moods]]''' are [[diffuse]] affective states that generally last for much longer durations than emotions and are also usually less intense than emotions. |
|||
* '''[[Affect (psychology)|Affect]]''' is an encompassing term, used to describe the topics of emotion, feelings, and moods together, even though it is commonly used interchangeably with emotion. |
|||
In addition, relationships exist between emotions, such as having positive or negative influences, with direct opposites existing. These concepts are described in [[contrasting and categorization of emotions]]. Graham differentiates emotions as functional or dysfunctional and argues all functional emotions have benefits.<ref>{{cite book|last1=Graham|first1=Michael C.|title=Facts of Life: ten issues of contentment|date=2014|publisher=Outskirts Press|ISBN=978-1-4787-2259-5}}</ref> |
|||
==Components== |
|||
In [[Klaus Scherer|Scherer's]] components processing model of emotion,<ref>{{cite journal |last=Scherer |first=K. R. |title=What are emotions? And how can they be measured? |journal=Social Science Information |year=2005 |volume=44 |pages=693–727}}</ref> five crucial elements of emotion are said to exist. From the component processing perspective, emotion experience is said to require that all of these processes become coordinated and synchronized for a short period of time, driven by appraisal processes. Although the inclusion of [[cognitive appraisal]] as one of the elements is slightly controversial, since some theorists make the assumption that emotion and [[cognition]] are separate but interacting systems, the component processing model provides a sequence of events that effectively describes the coordination involved during an emotional episode. |
|||
* '''Cognitive appraisal:''' provides an evaluation of events and objects |
|||
* '''Bodily symptoms:''' the [[physiological]] component of emotional experience |
|||
* '''Action tendencies:''' a [[motivation]]al component for the preparation and direction of motor responses. |
|||
* '''Expression:''' [[facial expressions|facial]] and [[vocal]] expression almost always accompanies an emotional state to communicate reaction and intention of actions |
|||
* '''Feelings:''' the subjective experience of emotional state once it has occurred |
|||
==Classification== |
|||
{{Main|Emotion classification}} |
|||
A distinction can be made between emotional episodes and emotional dispositions. Emotional dispositions are also comparable to character traits, where someone may be said to be generally disposed to experience certain emotions. For example, an irritable person is generally disposed to feel [[irritation]] more easily or quickly than others do. Finally, some theorists place emotions within a more general category of "affective states" where affective states can also include emotion-related phenomena such as pleasure and pain, motivational states (for example, [[hunger]] or [[curiosity]]), moods, dispositions and traits.<ref>Schwarz, N. H. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, 2, 527-561.</ref> |
|||
The classification of emotions has mainly been researched from two fundamental viewpoints. The first viewpoint is that emotions are discrete and fundamentally different constructs while the second viewpoint asserts that emotions can be characterized on a dimensional basis in groupings. |
|||
===Basic emotions=== |
|||
[[File:Emotions_-_3.png|thumb|Examples of basic emotions]] |
|||
For more than 40 years, [[Paul Ekman]] has supported the view that emotions are discrete, measurable, and physiologically distinct. Ekman's most influential work revolved around the finding that certain emotions appeared to be universally recognized, even in cultures that were preliterate and could not have learned associations for facial expressions through media. Another classic study found that when participants contorted their facial muscles into distinct facial expressions (e.g. disgust), they reported subjective and physiological experiences that matched the distinct facial expressions. His research findings led him to classify six emotions as basic: [[anger]], [[disgust]], [[fear]], [[happiness]], [[sadness]] and [[surprise (emotion)|surprise]].<ref name=Handel>{{cite web |last=Handel |first=Steven |title=Classification of Emotions |url=http://www.theemotionmachine.com/classification-of-emotions |accessdate=30 April 2012}}</ref> |
|||
[[Robert Plutchik]] agreed with Ekman's biologically driven perspective but developed the "wheel of emotions", suggesting eight primary emotions grouped on a positive or negative basis: joy versus sadness; anger versus fear; trust versus distrust; and surprise versus anticipation.<ref name="Handel"/> Some basic emotions can be modified to form complex emotions. The complex emotions could arise from cultural conditioning or association combined with the basic emotions. Alternatively, similar to the way [[primary color]]s combine, ''primary emotions'' could blend to form the full spectrum of human emotional experience. For example, interpersonal [[anger]] and [[disgust]] could blend to form [[contempt]]. Relationships exist between basic emotions, resulting in positive or negative influences.<ref>Plutchik , R. (2002). Nature of emotions" ''American Scientist''89, 349.</ref> |
|||
===Multi-dimensional analysis=== |
|||
[[File:Two Dimensions of Emotion.gif.jpg|.5px|thumbnail|left|Two Dimensions of Emotion]] |
|||
Through the use of [[multidimensional scaling]], psychologists can map out similar emotional experiences, which allows a visual depiction of the "emotional distance" between experiences. A further step can be taken by looking at the map's dimensions of the emotional experiences. The emotional experiences are divided into two dimensions known as [[Valence (psychology)|valences]] (how negative or positive the experience was) and [[arousal]] (extent of reaction to stimuli). These two dimensions can be depicted on a 2D coordinate map.<ref name="<ref>{{cite book|last=Schacter|first=Daniel L.|title=Psychology Ed. 2|year=2011|publisher=Worth Publishers|location=41 Madison Avenue New York, NY 10010|isbn=1-4292-3719-8}}</ref> |
|||
==Theories{{anchor|theory}} on the experience==<!--Facial feedback--> |
|||
===Ancient Greece and Middle Ages=== |
|||
Theories about emotions stretch back to at least as far as the stoics of Ancient Greece and Ancient China. In the latter it was believed that excess emotion caused damage to ''[[qi]]'', which in turn, damages the vital organs.<ref name="Suchy 2011">{{cite book|last=Suchy|first=Yana|title=Clinical neuropsychology of emotion|year=2011|publisher=Guilford|location=New York, NY}}</ref> The four humours theory made popular by [[Hippocrates]] contributed to the study of emotion in the same way that it did for [[medicine]]. |
|||
[[Western philosophy]] regarded emotion in varying ways. In [[stoicism|stoic]] theories it was seen as a hindrance to [[reason]] and therefore a hindrance to virtue. [[Aristotle]] believed that emotions were an essential component to [[virtue]].<ref>{{cite book|last=Aristotle|title=Nicomachean Ethics|location=Book 2. Chapter 6|url=http://www.constitution.org/ari/ethic_02.htm#2.6}}</ref> In the Aristotelian view all emotions (called passions) corresponded to an appetite or capacity. During the [[Middle Ages]], the Aristotelian view was adopted and further developed by [[scholasticism]] and [[Thomas Aquinas]]<ref>{{cite book|last=Aquinas|first=Thomas|title=Summa Theologica|location=Q.59, Art.2|url=http://www.newadvent.org/summa/2059.htm}}</ref> in particular. There are also theories in the works of philosophers such as [[René Descartes]], [[Niccolò Machiavelli]], [[Baruch Spinoza]]<ref>See for instance Antonio Damasio (2005) ''Looking for Spinoza''.</ref> and [[David Hume]]. In the 19th century emotions were considered adaptive and were studied more frequently from an [[empiricist]] psychiatric perspective. |
|||
===Evolutionary theories=== |
|||
{{Main|Evolution of emotion|Evolutionary psychology}} |
|||
[[File:Expression of the Emotions Figure 15.png|thumb|Illustration from [[Charles Darwin]]'s ''[[The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals]]''.]] |
|||
;19th Century |
|||
Perspectives on emotions from evolutionary theory were initiated in the late 19th century with [[Charles Darwin]]'s book ''[[The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals]]''.<ref>Darwin, Charles (1872). ''The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals''. Note: This book was originally published in 1872, but has been reprinted many times thereafter by different publishers</ref> Darwin argued that emotions actually served a purpose for humans, in communication and also in aiding their survival. Darwin, therefore, argued that emotions evolved via [[natural selection]] and therefore have universal [[cross-cultural]] counterparts. Darwin also detailed the virtues of experiencing emotions and the parallel experiences that occur in animals (see [[emotion in animals]]). This led the way for animal research on emotions and the eventual determination of the neural underpinnings of emotion. |
|||
;Contemporary |
|||
More contemporary views along the [[evolutionary psychology]] spectrum posit that both basic emotions and social emotions evolved to motivate (social) behaviors that were adaptive in the ancestral environment.<ref name = "Gaulin 6"/> Current research{{citation needed|date=July 2013}} suggests that emotion is an essential part of any human decision-making and planning, and the famous distinction made between reason and emotion is not as clear as it seems. Paul D. MacLean claims that emotion competes with even more instinctive responses, on one hand, and the more abstract reasoning, on the other hand. The increased potential in [[neuroimaging]] has also allowed investigation into evolutionarily ancient parts of the brain. Important neurological advances were derived from these perspectives in the 1990s by [[Joseph E. LeDoux]] and [[António Damásio]]. |
|||
Research on social emotion also focuses on the physical displays of emotion including body language of animals and humans (see [[affect display]]). For example, spite seems to work against the individual but it can establish an individual's reputation as someone to be feared.<ref name = "Gaulin 6"/> Shame and pride can motivate behaviors that help one maintain one's standing in a community, and self-esteem is one's estimate of one's status.<ref name = "Gaulin 6"/><ref>Wright, Robert. Moral animal.</ref> |
|||
===Somatic theories=== |
|||
[[Somatic marker hypothesis|Somatic]] theories of emotion claim that bodily responses, rather than cognitive interpretations, are essential to emotions. The first modern version of such theories came from William James in the 1880s. The theory lost favor in the 20th century, but has regained popularity more recently due largely to theorists such as [[John Cacioppo]],<ref>Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Somatic responses to psychological stress: The reactivity hypothesis. Advances in psychological science, Vol. 2, pp. 87-114. East Sussex, United Kingdom: Psychology Press</ref> [[António Damásio]],<ref>Aziz-Zadeh L, Damasio A. (2008) Embodied semantics for actions: findings from functional brain imaging. J Physiol Paris. Jan-May;102(1-3):35-9</ref> [[Joseph E. LeDoux]]<ref>LeDoux J.E. (1996) The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.</ref> and [[Robert Zajonc]]<ref>McIntosh, D. N., Zajonc, R. B., Vig, P. S., & Emerick, S. W. (1997). Facial movement, breathing, temperature, and affect: Implications of the vascular theory of emotional efference. Cognition & Emotion, 11(2), 171-195.</ref> who are able to appeal to neurological evidence.{{Citation needed|date=February 2011}} |
|||
====James–Lange theory==== |
|||
{{Main|James–Lange theory}} |
|||
In his 1884 article<ref name=James>[[William James|James, William]]. 1884. [http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/James/emotion.htm "What Is an Emotion?"] ''[[Mind (journal)|Mind]]''. 9, no. 34: 188-205.</ref> [[William James]] argued that feelings and emotions were ''secondary'' to [[physiological]] phenomena. In his theory, James proposed that the perception of what he called an "exciting fact" led directly to a physiological response, known as "emotion." To account for different types of emotional experiences, James proposed that stimuli trigger activity in the autonomic nervous system, which in turn produces an emotional experience in the brain. The Danish psychologist [[Carl Lange (physician)|Carl Lange]] also proposed a similar theory at around the same time, and therefore this theory became known as the [[James–Lange theory]]. As James wrote, "the perception of bodily changes, as they occur, ''is'' the emotion." James further claims that "we feel sad because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble, and neither we cry, strike, nor tremble because we are sorry, angry, or fearful, as the case may be."<ref name=James/> |
|||
An example of this theory in action would be as follows: An emotion-evoking stimulus (snake) triggers a pattern of physiological response (increased heart rate, faster breathing, etc.), which is interpreted as a particular emotion (fear). This theory is supported by experiments in which by manipulating the bodily state induces a desired emotional state.<ref>Laird, James, ''Feelings: the Perception of Self'', Oxford University Press</ref> Some people may believe that emotions give rise to emotion-specific actions: e.g. "I'm crying because I'm sad," or "I ran away because I was scared." The issue with the James–Lange theory is that of causation (bodily states causing emotions and being ''a priori''), not that of the bodily influences on emotional experience (which can be argued and is still quite prevalent today in biofeedback studies and embodiment theory).<ref>Reisenzein, R. (1995). James and the physical basis of emotion: A comment on ellsworth. Psychological review, 102(4), 757-761. {{DOI|10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.757}}</ref> |
|||
Although mostly abandoned in its original form, Tim Dalgleish argues that most contemporary neuroscientists have embraced the components of the James-Lange theory of emotions.<ref>Dalgleish, T. (2004). The emotional brain. ''Nature: Perspectives'', 5, 582–89.</ref> |
|||
{{quote|The James–Lange theory has remained influential. Its main contribution is the emphasis it places on the embodiment of emotions, especially the argument that changes in the bodily concomitants of emotions can alter their experienced intensity. Most contemporary neuroscientists would endorse a modified James–Lange view in which bodily feedback modulates the experience of emotion." (p. 583)}} |
|||
====Cannon–Bard theory==== |
|||
{{Main|Cannon–Bard theory}} |
|||
[[Walter Bradford Cannon]] agreed that physiological responses played a crucial role in emotions, but did not believe that physiological responses alone could explain [[subjectivity|subjective]] emotional experiences. He argued that physiological responses were too slow and often imperceptible and this could not account for the relatively rapid and intense subjective awareness of emotion. He also believed that the richness, variety, and temporal course of emotional experiences could not stem from physiological reactions, that reflected fairly undifferentiated fight or flight responses.<ref name="Cannon 1929 399–421">{{cite journal|last=Cannon|first=Walter B.|title=Organization for Physiological Homeostasis|journal=Physiological Review|year=1929|volume=9|issue=3|pages=399–421}}</ref><ref>{{cite journal|last=Cannon|first=Walter B.|title=The James-Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and an alternative theory.|journal=The American Journal of Psychology|year=1927|volume=39|pages=106–124|doi=10.2307/1415404}}</ref> An example of this theory in action is as follows: An emotion-evoking event (snake) triggers simultaneously both a physiological response and a conscious experience of an emotion. |
|||
Phillip Bard contributed to the theory with his work on animals. Bard found that sensory, motor, and physiological information all had to pass through the [[diencephalon]] (particularly the [[thalamus]]), before being subjected to any further processing. Therefore, Cannon also argued that it was not anatomically possible for sensory events to trigger a physiological response prior to triggering conscious awareness and emotional stimuli had to trigger both physiological and experiential aspects of emotion simultaneously.<ref name="Cannon 1929 399–421"/> |
|||
====Two-factor theory==== |
|||
{{Main|Two-factor theory of emotion}} |
|||
[[Stanley Schachter]] formulated his theory on the earlier work of a Spanish physician, [[Gregorio Maranon]], who injected patients with [[epinephrine]] and subsequently asked them how they felt. Interestingly, Maranon found that most of these patients felt something but in the absence of an actual emotion-evoking stimulus, the patients were unable to interpret their physiological arousal as an experienced emotion. Schachter did agree that physiological reactions played a big role in emotions. He suggested that physiological reactions contributed to emotional experience by facilitating a focused cognitive appraisal of a given physiologically arousing event and that this appraisal was what defined the subjective emotional experience. Emotions were thus a result of two-stage process: general physiological arousal, and experience of emotion. For example, the physiological arousal, heart pounding, in a response to an evoking stimulus, the sight of a bear in the kitchen. The brain then quickly scans the area, to explain the pounding, and notices the bear. Consequently, the brain interprets the pounding heart as being the result of fearing the bear.<ref>{{cite book| authors=Daniel L. Schacter, Daniel T. Gilbert, Daniel M. Wegner |title=Psychology |year=2011 |publisher=Worth Publishers )}}</ref> With his student, [[Jerome Singer]], Schachter demonstrated that subjects can have different emotional reactions despite being placed into the same physiological state with an injection of epinephrine. Subjects were observed to express either anger or amusement depending on whether another person in the situation (a confederate) displayed that emotion. Hence, the combination of the appraisal of the situation (cognitive) and the participants' reception of adrenaline or a placebo together determined the response. This experiment has been criticized in Jesse Prinz's (2004) ''Gut Reactions''. |
|||
===Cognitive theories=== |
|||
With the two-factor theory now incorporating cognition, several theories began to argue that cognitive activity in the form of judgments, evaluations, or thoughts were entirely necessary for an emotion to occur. One of the main proponents of this view was [[Richard Lazarus]] who argued that emotions must have some cognitive [[intentionality]]. The cognitive activity involved in the interpretation of an emotional context may be conscious or unconscious and may or may not take the form of conceptual processing. |
|||
Lazarus' theory is very influential; emotion is a disturbance that occurs in the following order: |
|||
# Cognitive appraisal—The individual assesses the event cognitively, which cues the emotion. |
|||
# Physiological changes—The cognitive reaction starts biological changes such as increased heart rate or pituitary adrenal response. |
|||
# Action—The individual feels the emotion and chooses how to react. |
|||
For example: Jenny sees a snake. |
|||
# Jenny cognitively assesses the snake in her presence. Cognition allows her to understand it as a danger. |
|||
# Her brain activates Adrenaline gland which pumps Adrenaline through her blood stream resulting in increased heartbeat. |
|||
# Jenny screams and runs away. |
|||
Lazarus stressed that the quality and intensity of emotions are controlled through cognitive processes. These processes underline coping strategies that form the emotional reaction by altering the relationship between the person and the environment. |
|||
[[George Mandler]] provided an extensive theoretical and empirical discussion of emotion as influenced by cognition, consciousness, and the autonomic nervous system in two books (Mind and Emotion, 1975, and Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress, 1984) |
|||
There are some theories on emotions arguing that cognitive activity in the form of judgements, evaluations, or thoughts are necessary in order for an emotion to occur. |
|||
A prominent philosophical exponent is [[Robert C. Solomon]] (for example, ''The Passions, Emotions and the Meaning of Life'', 1993). Solomon claims that emotions are judgements. He has put forward a more nuanced view which responds to what he has called the ‘standard objection’ to cognitivism, the idea that a judgement that something is fearsome can occur with or without emotion, so judgement cannot be identified with emotion. |
|||
The theory proposed by [[Nico Frijda]] where appraisal leads to action tendencies is another example. |
|||
It has also been suggested that emotions (affect heuristics, feelings and gut-feeling reactions) are often used as shortcuts to process information and influence behavior.<ref>see the Heuristic–Systematic Model, or HSM, (Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989) under [[attitude change]]. Also see the index entry for "Emotion" in "Beyond Rationality: The Search for Wisdom in a Troubled Time" by Kenneth R. Hammond and in "Fooled by Randomness: The Hidden Role of Chance in Life and in the Markets" by Nassim Nicholas Taleb.</ref> The [[affect infusion model]] (AIM) is a theoretical model developed by Joseph Forgas in the early 1990s that attempts to explain how emotion and mood interact with one's ability to process information. |
|||
;Perceptual theory |
|||
Theories dealing with perception either use one or multiples perceptions in order to find an emotion (Goldie, 2007).A recent hybrid of the somatic and cognitive theories of emotion is the perceptual theory. This theory is neo-Jamesian in arguing that bodily responses are central to emotions, yet it emphasizes the meaningfulness of emotions or the idea that emotions are about something, as is recognized by cognitive theories. The novel claim of this theory is that conceptually-based cognition is unnecessary for such meaning. Rather the bodily changes themselves ''perceive'' the meaningful content of the emotion because of being causally triggered by certain situations. In this respect, emotions are held to be analogous to faculties such as vision or touch, which provide information about the relation between the subject and the world in various ways. A sophisticated defense of this view is found in philosopher Jesse Prinz's book ''Gut Reactions'', and psychologist James Laird's book ''Feelings''. |
|||
;Affective events theory |
|||
This is a communication-based theory developed by Howard M. Weiss and Russell Cropanzano (1996), that looks at the causes, structures, and consequences of emotional experience (especially in work contexts). This theory suggests that emotions are influenced and caused by events which in turn influence attitudes and behaviors. This theoretical frame also emphasizes ''time'' in that human beings experience what they call emotion episodes— a "series of emotional states extended over time and organized around an underlying theme." This theory has been utilized by numerous researchers to better understand emotion from a communicative lens, and was reviewed further by Howard M. Weiss and Daniel J. Beal in their article, "Reflections on Affective Events Theory", published in ''Research on Emotion in Organizations'' in 2005. |
|||
===Situated perspective on emotion=== |
|||
A situated perspective on emotion, developed by Paul E. Griffiths and Andrea Scarantino , emphasizes the importance of external factors in the development and communication of emotion, drawing upon the [[Situationism (psychology)|situationism]] approach in psychology.<ref>Griffiths, Paul Edmund and Scarantino, Andrea (2005) Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion.</ref> This theory is markedly different from both cognitivist and neo-Jamesian theories of emotion, both of which see emotion as a purely internal process, with the environment only acting as a stimulus to the emotion. In contrast, a situationist perspective on emotion views emotion as the product of an organism investigating its environment, and observing the responses of other organisms. Emotion stimulates the evolution of social relationships, acting as a signal to mediate the behavior of other organisms. In some contexts, the expression of emotion (both voluntary and involuntary) could be seen as strategic moves in the transactions between different organisms. The situated perspective on emotion states that conceptual thought is not an inherent part of emotion, since emotion is an action-oriented form of skillful engagement with the world. Griffiths and Scarantino suggested that this perspective on emotion could be helpful in understanding phobias, as well as the emotions of infants and animals. |
|||
==Genetics== |
|||
Emotions can motivate social interactions and relationships and therefore are directly related with basic physiology, particularly with the stress systems. This is important because emotions are related to the anti-stress complex, with an oxytocin-attachment system, which plays a major role in bonding. Emotional phenotype temperaments affect social connectedness and fitness in complex social systems (Kurt Kortschal 2013). These characteristics are shared with other species and taxa and are due to the effects of genes and their continuous transmission. Information that is encoded in the DNA sequences provides the blueprint for assembling proteins that make up our cells. Zygotes require genetic information from their parental germ cells, and at every speciation event, heritable traits that have enabled its’ ancestor to survive and reproduce successfully are passed down along with new traits that could be potentially beneficial to the offspring. |
|||
In the five million years since the linages leading to modern humans and chimpanzees split, only about 1.2% of their genetic material has been modified. This suggests that everything that separates us from chimpanzees must be encoded in that very small amount of DNA, including our behaviors. Students that study animal behaviors have only identified intraspecific examples of gene-dependent behavioral phenotypes. In voles (Microtus spp.) minor genetic differences have been identified in a vasopressin receptor gene that corresponds to major species differences in social organization and the mating system (Hammock & Young 2005). |
|||
Another potential example with behavioral differences is the FOCP2 gene, which is involved in neural circuitry handling speech and language (Vargha-Khadem et al. 2005). Its present form in humans differed from that of the chimpanzees by only a few mutations and has been present for about 200,000 years, coinciding with the beginning of modern humans (Enard et al. 2002). Speech, language, and social organization are all part of the basis for emotions. |
|||
==Neurocircuitry== |
|||
Based on discoveries made through neural mapping of the [[limbic system]], the [[neurobiological]] explanation of human emotion is that emotion is a pleasant or unpleasant mental state organized in the limbic system of the [[mammalian brain]]. If distinguished from reactive responses of [[reptile]]s, emotions would then be mammalian elaborations of general [[vertebrate]] arousal patterns, in which neurochemicals (for example, [[dopamine]], [[noradrenaline]], and [[serotonin]]) step-up or step-down the brain's activity level, as visible in body movements, gestures, and postures. Emotions can likely be mediated by [[pheromones]] (see [[fear]]).<ref name="Givens">{{cite web|last=Givens|first=David B. (1998)|title=Emotion |url=http://center-for-nonverbal-studies.org/emotion.htm|work=Center for Nonverbal Studies|accessdate=7 May 2014}}</ref> |
|||
For example, the emotion of [[love]] is proposed to be the expression of [[paleocircuits]] of the mammalian brain (specifically, modules of the [[cingulate gyrus]]) which facilitate the care, feeding, and grooming of offspring. Paleocircuits are neural platforms for bodily expression configured before the advent of [[Cerebral cortex|cortical]] [[neuronal circuit|circuits]] for speech. They consist of pre-configured pathways or networks of [[nerve cell]]s in the [[forebrain]], [[brain stem]] and [[spinal cord]]. |
|||
The motor centers of [[reptiles]] react to sensory cues of vision, sound, touch, chemical, gravity, and motion with pre-set body movements and programmed postures. With the arrival of night-active [[mammal]]s, smell replaced vision as the dominant sense, and a different way of responding arose from the [[olfactory]] sense, which is proposed to have developed into [[mammal]]ian emotion and emotional memory. The mammalian brain invested heavily in [[olfaction]] to succeed at night as reptiles slept—one explanation for why olfactory lobes in mammalian brains are proportionally larger than in the reptiles. These odor pathways gradually formed the neural blueprint for what was later to become our limbic brain.<ref name="Givens"/> |
|||
[[File:Lövheim cube of emotion.svg|thumb|[[Lövheim cube of emotion]]]] |
|||
Emotions are thought to be related to certain activities in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and determine the significance of what is going on around us. Pioneering work by [[Paul Broca|Broca]] (1878), [[James Papez|Papez]] (1937), and [[Paul D. MacLean|MacLean]] (1952) suggested that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the [[limbic system]], which includes the [[hypothalamus]], [[cingulate cortex]], [[hippocampi]], and other structures. More recent research has shown that some of these [[limbic system|limbic structures]] are not as directly related to emotion as others are, while some non-limbic structures have been found to be of greater emotional relevance. |
|||
In 2011, Lövheim proposed a direct relation between specific combinations of the levels of the signal substances [[dopamine]], [[noradrenaline]] and [[serotonin]] and eight basic emotions. A model was presented where the signal substances form the axes of a coordinate system, and the eight basic emotions according to [[Silvan Tomkins]] are placed in the eight corners. Anger is, according to the model, for example produced by the combination of low serotonin, high dopamine and high noradrenaline.<ref>Lövheim H. A new three-dimensional model for emotions and monoamine neurotransmitters. Med Hypotheses (2011), Epub ahead of print. {{doi|10.1016/j.mehy.2011.11.016}} PMID 22153577</ref> |
|||
===Prefrontal cortex=== |
|||
There is ample evidence that the left prefrontal cortex is activated by stimuli that cause positive approach.<ref>Kringelbach, M.L.; O'Doherty, J.O.; Rolls, E.T.; & Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. ''Cerebral Cortex'', 13, 1064–1071.</ref> If attractive stimuli can selectively activate a region of the brain, then logically the converse should hold, that selective activation of that region of the brain should cause a stimulus to be judged more positively. This was demonstrated for moderately attractive visual stimuli<ref>Drake, R.A. (1987). Effects of gaze manipulation on aesthetic judgments: Hemisphere priming of affect. ''Acta Psychologica'', 65, 91–99.</ref> and replicated and extended to include negative stimuli.<ref>Merckelbach, H.; & van Oppen, P. (1989). Effects of gaze manipulation on subjective evaluation of neutral and phobia-relevant stimuli: A comment on Drake's (1987) 'Effects of gaze manipulation on aesthetic judgments: Hemisphere priming of affect.' ''Acta Psychologica'', 70, 147–151.</ref> |
|||
Two neurobiological models of emotion in the prefrontal cortex made opposing predictions. The Valence Model predicted that anger, a negative emotion, would activate the right prefrontal cortex. The Direction Model predicted that anger, an approach emotion, would activate the left prefrontal cortex. The second model was supported.<ref>Harmon-Jones, E.; Vaughn-Scott, K.; Mohr, S.; Sigelman, J.; & Harmon-Jones, C. (2004). The effect of manipulated sympathy and anger on left and right frontal cortical activity. ''Emotion'', 4, 95–101.</ref> |
|||
This still left open the question of whether the opposite of approach in the prefrontal cortex is better described as moving away (Direction Model), as unmoving but with strength and resistance (Movement Model), or as unmoving with passive yielding (Action Tendency Model). Support for the Action Tendency Model (passivity related to right prefrontal activity) comes from research on shyness<ref>Schmidt, L.A. (1999). Frontal brain electrical activity in shyness and sociability. ''Psychological Science'', 10, 316–320.</ref> and research on behavioral inhibition.<ref>Garavan, H.; Ross, T.J.; & Stein, E.A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. ''Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences'', 96, 8301–8306.</ref> Research that tested the competing hypotheses generated by all four models also supported the Action Tendency Model.<ref>Drake, R.A.; & Myers, L.R. (2006). Visual attention, emotion, and action tendency: Feeling active or passive. ''Cognition and Emotion'', 20, 608–622.</ref><ref>Wacker, J.; Chavanon, M.-L.; Leue, A.; & Stemmler, G. (2008). Is running away right? The behavioral activation–behavioral inhibition model of anterior asymmetry. ''Emotion'', 8, 232–249.</ref> |
|||
{{anchor|Homeostatic emotion}} |
|||
===Homeostatic/primordial emotion=== |
|||
Another neurological approach distinguishes two classes of emotion: "classical" emotions such as love, anger and fear that are evoked by environmental stimuli, and "primordial" or "[[homeostatic emotion]]s" – attention-demanding feelings evoked by body states, such as pain, hunger and fatigue, that motivate behavior (withdrawal, eating or resting in these examples) aimed at maintaining the body's internal milieu at its ideal state.<ref name=Craig2003>{{Cite journal |last=[http://www.cba.arizona.edu/People/Faculty/Craig-A.htm Craig] |first=A.D. (Bud) |year= 2003 |title=Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body |journal=Current Opinion in Neurobiology |volume=13 |pages=500–505 |pmid=12965300 |doi=10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4 |url= http://www.jsmf.org/meetings/2007/oct-nov/CONB%20Craig%202003.pdf |issue=4}}</ref> |
|||
[[Derek Denton]] defines the latter as "the subjective element of the instincts, which are the genetically programmed behaviour patterns which contrive [[Human homeostasis|homeostasis]]. They include thirst, hunger for air, hunger for food, pain and hunger for specific minerals etc. There are two constituents of a primordial emotion--the specific sensation which when severe may be imperious, and the compelling intention for gratification by a consummatory act."<ref name="Denton2009">{{cite journal |author=Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Farrell M, Egan GF |title=The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness |journal=Conscious Cogn |volume=18 |issue=2 |pages=500–14 |date=June 2009 |pmid=18701321 |doi=10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.009 |url=}}</ref> |
|||
==Disciplinary approaches== |
|||
Many different disciplines have produced work on the emotions. [[Human sciences]] study the role of emotions in mental processes, disorders, and neural mechanisms. In [[psychiatry]], emotions are examined as part of the discipline's study and treatment of mental disorders in humans. [[Nursing]] studies emotions as part of its approach to the provision of holistic health care to humans. [[Psychology]] examines emotions from a scientific perspective by treating them as mental processes and behavior and they explore the underlying physiological and neurological processes. In [[neuroscience]] sub-fields such as [[social neuroscience]] and [[affective neuroscience]], scientists study the neural mechanisms of emotion by combining neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood. In [[linguistics]], the expression of emotion may change to the meaning of sounds. In [[education]], the role of emotions in relation to learning is examined. |
|||
[[Social sciences]] often examine emotion for the role that it plays in human culture and social interactions. In [[sociology]], emotions are examined for the role they play in human society, social patterns and interactions, and culture. In [[anthropology]], the study of humanity, scholars use ethnography to undertake contextual analyses and cross-cultural comparisons of a range of human activities. Some anthropology studies examine the role of emotions in human activities. In the field of [[communication sciences]], critical organizational scholars have examined the role of emotions in organizations, from the perspectives of managers, employees, and even customers. A focus on emotions in organizations can be credited to [[Arlie Russell Hochschild]]'s concept of [[emotional labor]]. The University of Queensland hosts EmoNet,<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.uq.edu.au/emonet/ |title=EmoNet |publisher=Uq.edu.au |accessdate=2013-11-11}}</ref> an e-mail distribution list representing a network of academics that facilitates scholarly discussion of all matters relating to the study of emotion in organizational settings. The list was established in January 1997 and has over 700 members from across the globe. |
|||
In [[economics]], the social science that studies the production, distribution, and consumption of goods and services, emotions are analyzed in some sub-fields of microeconomics, in order to assess the role of emotions on purchase decision-making and risk perception. In [[criminology]], a social science approach to the study of crime, scholars often draw on behavioral sciences, sociology, and psychology; emotions are examined in criminology issues such as [[anomie]] theory and studies of "toughness," aggressive behavior, and hooliganism. In [[law]], which underpins civil obedience, politics, economics and society, evidence about people's emotions is often raised in [[tort law]] claims for compensation and in [[criminal law]] prosecutions against alleged lawbreakers (as evidence of the defendant's state of mind during trials, sentencing, and parole hearings). In [[political science]], emotions are examined in a number of sub-fields, such as the analysis of voter decision-making. |
|||
In [[philosophy]], emotions are studied in sub-fields such as [[ethics]], the [[Aesthetics|philosophy of art]] (for example, [[sensory–emotional]] values, and matters of [[Taste (sociology)|taste]] and [[sentimentality]]), and the [[philosophy of music]] (see also [[Music and emotion]]). In [[history]], scholars examine documents and other sources to interpret and analyze past activities; speculation on the emotional state of the authors of historical documents is one of the tools of interpretation. In [[literature]] and film-making, the expression of emotion is the cornerstone of genres such as drama, melodrama, and romance. In [[communication studies]], scholars study the role that emotion plays in the dissemination of ideas and messages. Emotion is also studied in non-human animals in [[ethology]], a branch of zoology which focuses on the scientific study of animal behavior. Ethology is a combination of laboratory and field science, with strong ties to ecology and evolution. Ethologists often study one type of behavior (for example, [[aggression]]) in a number of unrelated animals. |
|||
===History=== |
|||
The [[history of emotions]] has become an increasingly popular topic recently, with some scholars arguing that it is an essential category of analysis, not unlike class, race, or gender. Historians, like other social scientists, assume that emotions, feelings and their expressions are regulated in different ways by both different cultures and different historical times, and [[social constructivism|constructivist]] school of history claims even that some sentiments and [[meta-emotions]], for example [[Schadenfreude]], are learnt and not only regulated by culture. Historians of emotion trace and analyse the changing norms and rules of feeling, while examining emotional regimes, codes, and lexicons from social, cultural or political history perspectives. Others focus on the history of medicine, science or psychology. What somebody can and may feel (and show) in a given situation, towards certain people or things, depends on social norms and rules. It is thus historically variable and open to change.<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.mpib-berlin.mpg.de/en/research/history-of-emotions |title=History of Emotions | Max Planck Institute for Human Development |publisher=Mpib-berlin.mpg.de |accessdate=2013-11-11}}</ref> Several research centers have opened in the past few years in Germany, England, Spain,<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.unav.es/centro/cemid/ |title=Cultura Emocional E Identidad |publisher=Unav.es |accessdate=2013-11-11}}</ref> Sweden and Australia. |
|||
Furthermore, research in [[historical trauma]] suggests that some [[Psychological trauma|traumatic]] emotions can be passed on from parents to offspring to second and even third generation, presented as examples of [[transgenerational trauma]]. |
|||
===Sociology=== |
|||
{{Main|Sociology of emotions}} |
|||
A common way in which emotions are conceptualized in sociology is in terms of the multidimensional characteristics including cultural or emotional labels (e.g., anger, pride, fear, happiness), physiological changes (e.g., increased perspiration, changes in pulse rate), expressive facial and body movements (e.g., smiling, frowning, baring teeth), and appraisals of situational cues.<ref name="Thoits, P. A. 1989"/> One comprehensive theory of emotional arousal in humans has been developed by Jonathan Turner (2007: 2009).<ref>Turner, J. H. (2007). Human emotions: A sociological theory. London: Routledge.</ref><ref>Turner, J. H. (2009). The sociology of emotion: Basic Theoretical arguments. Emotion Review, 1(4), 340-354.</ref> Two of the key eliciting factors for the arousal of emotions within this theory are expectations states and sanctions. When people enter a situation or encounter with certain expectations for how the encounter should unfold, they will experience different emotions depending on the extent to which expectations for Self, other and situation are met or not met. People can also provide positive or negative sanctions directed at Self or other which also trigger different emotional experiences in individuals. Turner analyzed a wide range of emotion theories across different fields of research including sociology, psychology, evolutionary science, and neuroscience. Based on this analysis, he identified four emotion that all researchers consider to founded on human neurology including assertive-anger, aversion-fear, satisfaction-happiness, and disappointment-sadness. These four categories are called primary emotions and there is some agreement amongst researchers that these primary emotions become combined to produce more elaborate and complex emotional experiences. These more elaborate emotions are called first-order elaborations in Turner's theory and they include sentiments such as pride, triumph, and awe. Emotions can also be experienced at different levels of intensity so that feelings of concern are a low-intensity variation of the primary emotion aversion-fear whereas depression is a higher intensity variant. |
|||
Attempts are frequently made to regulate emotion according to the conventions of the society and the situation based on many (sometimes conflicting) demands and expectations which originate from various entities. The emotion of anger is in many cultures discouraged in girls and women, while fear is discouraged in boys and men. Expectations attached to social roles, such as "acting as man" and not as a woman, and the accompanying "feeling rules" contribute to the differences in expression of certain emotions. Some cultures encourage or discourage happiness, sadness, or jealousy, and the free expression of the emotion of disgust is considered socially unacceptable in most cultures. Some social institutions are seen as based on certain emotion, such as [[love]] in the case of contemporary institution of [[marriage]]. In advertising, such as health campaigns and political messages, emotional appeals are commonly found. Recent examples include no-smoking health campaigns and political campaign advertising emphasizing the fear of terrorism. |
|||
Sociological attention to emotion has varied over time. Emilé Durkheim (1915/1965)<ref>Durkheim, E. (1915/1912). The elementary forms of the religious life, trans. J. W. Swain. New York: Free Press.</ref> wrote about the collective effervescence or emotional energy that was experienced by members of totemic rituals in Australian aborigine society. He explained how the heightened state of emotional energy achieved during totemic rituals transported individuals above themselves giving them the sense that they were in the presence of a higher power, a force, that was embedded in the sacred objects that were worshipped. These feelings of exaltation, he argued, ultimately lead people to believe that there were forces that governed sacred objects. |
|||
In the 1990s, sociologists focused on different aspects of specific emotions and how these emotions were socially relevant. For Cooley (1992),<ref>Cooley, C. H. (1992). Human nature and the social order. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.</ref> pride and shame were the most important emotions that drive people to take various social actions. During every encounter, he proposed that we monitor ourselves through the "looking glass" that the gestures and reactions of others provide. Depending on these reactions, we either experience pride or shame and this results in particular paths of action. Retzinger (1991)<ref>Retzinger, S. M. (1991). Violent emotions: Shame and rage in marital quarrels. London: SAGE.</ref> conducted studies of married couples who experienced cycles of rage and shame. Drawing predominantly on Goffman and Cooley's work, Scheff (1990)<ref>Scheff, J. (1990). Microsociology: discourse, emotion and social structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.</ref> developed a microsociological theory of the social bond. The formation or disruption of social bonds is dependant on the emotions that people experience during interactions. |
|||
Subsequent to these developments, Randall Collins (2004)<ref>Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.</ref> formulated his interaction ritual theory by drawing on Durkheim's work on totemic rituals that was extended by Goffman (1964/2013; 1967)<ref>Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual. New York: Anchor Books.</ref><ref>Goffman, E. (1964/2013). Encounters: Two studies in the sociology of interactions. Mansfiled Centre, CT: Martino Publishing.</ref> into everyday focused encounters. Based on interaction ritual theory, we experience different levels or intensities of emotional energy during face-to-face interactions. Emotional energy is considered to be a feeling of confidence to take action and a boldness that one experiences when they are charged up from the collective effervescence generated during group gatherings that reach high levels of intensity. |
|||
There is a growing body of research applying the sociology of emotion to understanding the learning experiences of school students during classroom interactions with teachers and other students (e.g., Milne & Otieno, 2007;<ref>Milne, C., & Otieno, T. (2007). Understanding engagement: Science demonstrations and emotional energy. Science Education, 91, 532-553. DOI 10.1002/sce</ref> Olitsky, 2007 |
|||
;<ref>Olitsky, S. (2007). Science learning, status and identity formation in an urban middle school. In W.-M. Roth & K. G. Tobin (Eds.), Science, learning, identity: Sociocultural and cultural-historical perspectives. (pp. 41-62). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense.</ref> Tobin, et al., 2013;<ref>Tobin, K., Ritchie, S. M., Oakley, J., Mergard, V., & Hudson, P. (2013). Relationships between emotional climate and the fluency of classroom interactions. Learning Environments Research, 16, 71-89.</ref> Zembylas, 2002<ref>Zembylas, M. (2002). Constructing genealogies of teachers’ emotions in science teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 79–103.</ref>). These studies show that learning subjects like school science can be understood in terms of classroom interaction rituals that generate emotional energy and collective states of emotional arousal like emotional climate. |
|||
Apart from interaction ritual traditions of the sociology of emotion, other approaches have been classed into one of 6 other categories (Turner, 2009) including: |
|||
# evolutionary/biological theories, |
|||
# symbolic interactionist theories, |
|||
# dramaturgical theories, |
|||
# ritual theories, |
|||
# power and status theories, |
|||
# stratification theories, and |
|||
# exchange theories. |
|||
This list provides a general overview of different traditions in the sociology of emotion that sometimes conceptualise emotion in different ways and at other times in complementary ways. Many of these different approaches were synthesized by Turner (2007) in his sociological theory of human emotions in an attempt to produce one comprehensive sociological account that draws on developments from many of the above traditions. |
|||
<ref>Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (2005) FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6:131–138</ref> |
|||
<ref>Enard W,KhaitovichP,KloseJ,Zo ̈llnerS,HeissigF,GiavaliscoP,Nieselt-StruweK,Much-more E, Varki A, Ravid R, Doxiadis GM, Bontrop RE, Pa ̈a ̈bo S (2002) Intra- and interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns. Science 296:340–343</ref> |
|||
<ref>Hammock E.A.D, Young L.J. Microsatellite instability generates diversity in brain and sociobehavioral traits. Science. 2005;308:1630–1634.</ref> |
|||
===Psychotherapy and regulation=== |
|||
Emotion regulation refers to the cognitive and behavioral strategies people use to influence their own emotional experience.<ref>Schacter, Daniel. "Psychology". Worth Publishers. 2011. p.316</ref> For example, a behavioral strategy in which one avoids a situation to avoid unwanted emotions (e.g., trying not to think about the situation, doing distracting activities, etc.).<ref>Schacter, Daniel. "Psychology". Worth Publishers. 2011. p.340</ref> Depending on the particular school's general emphasis on either cognitive components of emotion, physical energy discharging, or on symbolic movement and facial expression components of emotion,<ref>Freitas-Magalhães, A., & Castro, E. (2009). Facial Expression: The effect of the smile in the Treatment of Depression. Empirical Study with Portuguese Subjects. In A. Freitas-Magalhães (Ed.), ''Emotional Expression: The Brain and The Face'' (pp. 127–140). Porto: University Fernando Pessoa Press. ISBN 978-989-643-034-4</ref> different schools of [[psychotherapy]] approach the regulation of emotion differently. Cognitively oriented schools approach them via their cognitive components, such as [[rational emotive behavior therapy]]. Yet others approach emotions via symbolic movement and facial expression components (like in contemporary [[Gestalt therapy]]).<ref>{{cite web|url=http://www.123webpages.co.uk/user/index.php?user=mgc&pn=10713 |title=On Emotion – an article from Manchester Gestalt Centre website |publisher=123webpages.co.uk |accessdate=2013-11-11}}</ref> |
|||
===Computer science=== |
|||
{{Main|Affective computing}} |
|||
In the 2000s, research in computer science, engineering, psychology and neuroscience has been aimed at developing devices that recognize human [[affect (psychology)|affect]] display and model emotions.<ref>Fellous, Armony & LeDoux, 2002</ref> In computer science, [[affective computing]] is a branch of the study and development of [[artificial intelligence]] that deals with the design of systems and devices that can recognize, interpret, and process human emotions. It is an interdisciplinary field spanning [[computer science]]s, [[psychology]], and [[cognitive science]].<ref name=TaoTan>{{cite conference |first= Jianhua |last=Tao |author2=Tieniu Tan |title=Affective Computing: A Review |booktitle=Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction |title=[[Lecture Notes in Computer Science{{!}}LNCS]] |volume=3784 |pages= 981–995 |publisher=Springer |year=2005 |doi=10.1007/11573548}}</ref> While the origins of the field may be traced as far back as to early philosophical enquiries into [[Emotion#James–Lange theory|emotion]],<ref name=James/> the more modern branch of computer science originated with [[Rosalind Picard]]'s 1995 paper<ref>[http://affect.media.mit.edu/pdfs/95.picard.pdf "Affective Computing"] MIT Technical Report #321 ([http://vismod.media.mit.edu/pub/tech-reports/TR-321-ABSTRACT.html Abstract]), 1995</ref> on affective computing.<ref>{{cite web |url=http://ls12-www.cs.tu-dortmund.de//~fink/lectures/SS06/human-robot-interaction/Emotion-RecognitionAndSimulation.pdf |title=Recognition and Simulation of Emotions |accessdate=May 13, 2008 |last=Kleine-Cosack |first=Christian |date=October 2006 |format=PDF |quote=The introduction of emotion to computer science was done by Pickard (sic) who created the field of affective computing. |archiveurl=http://web.archive.org/web/20080528135730/http://ls12-www.cs.tu-dortmund.de/~fink/lectures/SS06/human-robot-interaction/Emotion-RecognitionAndSimulation.pdf <!--Bot retrieved archive--> |archivedate=May 28, 2008}}</ref><ref>{{cite web |url=http://www.wired.com/wired/archive/11.12/love.html |title=The Love Machine; Building computers that care. |accessdate=May 13, 2008 |last=Diamond |first=David |date=December 2003 |publisher=Wired |quote=Rosalind Picard, a genial [[MIT]] professor, is the field's godmother; her 1997 book, Affective Computing, triggered an explosion of interest in the emotional side of computers and their users.}}</ref> Detecting emotional information begins with passive [[sensor]]s which capture data about the user's physical state or behavior without interpreting the input. The data gathered is analogous to the cues humans use to perceive emotions in others. Another area within affective computing is the design of computational devices proposed to exhibit either innate emotional capabilities or that are capable of convincingly simulating emotions. Emotional speech processing recognizes the user's emotional state by analyzing speech patterns. The detection and processing of facial expression or body gestures is achieved through detectors and sensors. |
|||
==Notable theorists== |
|||
<!--Please do not expand this list without discussion first on the Talk Page. This should not be a long list of all scientists working on emotion; it should just include the most notable and most-cited scientists. Undiscussed additions will be removed.--> |
|||
[[File:William James b1842b.jpg|thumb|left|upright=0.56|[[William James]] about the time he wrote on emotions]] |
|||
In the late 19th century, the most influential theorists were [[William James]] (1842–1910) and [[Carl Lange (physician)|Carl Lange]] (1834–1900). James was an American psychologist and philosopher who wrote about educational psychology, psychology of religious experience/mysticism, and the philosophy of pragmatism. Lange was a Danish physician and psychologist. Working independently, they developed the [[James–Lange theory]], a hypothesis on the origin and nature of emotions. The theory states that within human beings, as a response to experiences in the world, the autonomic nervous system creates physiological events such as muscular tension, a rise in heart rate, perspiration, and dryness of the mouth. Emotions, then, are feelings which come about as a result of these physiological changes, rather than being their cause.<ref>{{cite web|last=Cherry|first=Kendra|title=v|url=http://psychology.about.com/od/jindex/g/jameslange.htm|accessdate=30 April 2012}}</ref> |
|||
[[Silvan Tomkins]] (1911–1991) developed the Affect theory and Script theory. The [[Affect theory]] introduced the concept of basic emotions, and was based on the idea that the dominance of the emotion , which he called the affect system, was the motivating force in human life.<ref>{{cite web|last=The Tomkins Institute|title=Applied Studies in Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition|url=http://www.tomkins.org/Tomkins.html|accessdate=30 April 2012}}</ref> |
|||
Some of the most influential theorists on emotion from the 20th century have died in the last decade. They include [[Magda B. Arnold]] (1903–2002), an American psychologist who developed the [[appraisal theory]] of emotions;<ref>Reisenzein , R. (2006). Cognition & emotion. Psychology Press, part of the Taylor & Francis Group, 20(7), 920-951. {{doi|10.1080/02699930600616445}}</ref> [[Richard Lazarus]] (1922–2002), an American psychologist who specialized in emotion and stress, especially in relation to cognition; [[Herbert A. Simon]] (1916–2001), who included emotions into decision making and artificial intelligence; [[Robert Plutchik]] (1928–2006), an American psychologist who developed a psychoevolutionary theory of emotion;<ref>Plutchik, R. (1982). A psychoevolutionary theory of emotions. Social Science Information, 21, 529. {{doi|10.1177/053901882021004003}}</ref> [[Robert Zajonc]] (1923–2008) a Polish–American social psychologist who specialized in social and cognitive processes such as social facilitation. An American philosopher, [[Robert C. Solomon]] (1942–2007), contributed to the theories on the philosophy of emotions with books such as ''What Is An Emotion?: Classic and Contemporary Readings'' (Oxford, 2003). [[Peter Goldie]] (1946–2011) British philosopher who specializes in ethics, aesthetics, emotion, mood and character |
|||
Influential theorists who are still active include the following psychologists, neurologists, philosophers, and sociologists: |
|||
* [[Lisa Feldman Barrett]] – Social philosopher and psychologist specializing in [[affective science]] and human emotion. |
|||
* [[John Cacioppo]] – from the [[University of Chicago]], founding father with [[Gary Berntson]] of [[social neuroscience]]. |
|||
* [[Randall Collins]] - (born 1941) American sociologist from the [[University of Pennsylvania]] developed the interaction ritual theory which includes emotional entrainment model |
|||
* [[António Damásio]] (born 1944) – Portuguese behavioral neurologist and neuroscientist who works in the US |
|||
* [[Richard Davidson]] (born 1951) – American psychologist and neuroscientist; pioneer in [[affective neuroscience]]. |
|||
* [[Paul Ekman]] (born 1934) – Psychologist specializing in the study of emotions and their relation to facial expressions |
|||
* [[Barbara Fredrickson]] – Social psychologist who specializes in emotions and positive psychology. |
|||
* [[Nico Frijda]] (born 1927) – Dutch psychologist who specializes in human emotions, especially facial expressions |
|||
* [[Arlie Russell Hochschild]] (born 1940) – American sociologist whose central contribution was in forging a link between the subcutaneous flow of emotion in social life and the larger trends set loose by modern capitalism within organizations. |
|||
* [[Joseph E. LeDoux]] (born 1949) – American neuroscientist who studies the biological underpinnings of memory and emotion, especially the mechanisms of fear |
|||
* [[George Mandler]] (born 1924) - American psychologist who wrote influential books on cognition and emotion |
|||
<!--Addition of the Portuguese professor Freitas-Magelhães without discussion on the talk page will be reverted as vandalism, this due to repeated addition of this name by a host of anonymous IP 195.xxxx editors.--> |
|||
* [[Jaak Panksepp]] (born 1943) – Estonian-born American psychologist, psychobiologist and neuroscientist; pioneer in affective neuroscience. |
|||
* [[Jesse Prinz]] – American philosopher who specializes in emotion, moral psychology, aesthetics and consciousness |
|||
* [[James A. Russell]] – American psychologist who developed the circumplex model of affect |
|||
* [[Klaus Scherer]] (born 1943) – Swiss psychologist and director of the Swiss Center for Affective Sciences in Geneva; he specializes in the psychology of emotion |
|||
* [[Ronald de Sousa]] (born 1940) – English–Canadian philosopher who specializes in the philosophy of emotions, philosophy of mind and philosophy of biology. |
|||
* [[Jonathan Turner]] (born 1942) - American sociologist from the [[University of California, Riverside]] who is a general sociological theorist with specialty areas including the sociology of emotions, ethnic relations, social institutions, social stratification, and bio-sociology. |
|||
* [[Dominique Moïsi]] (born 1946) - Authored a book titled <u>The Geopolitics of Emotion</u> focusing on emotions related to globalization. |
|||
==See also== |
|||
{{colbegin|colwidth=15em}} |
|||
* [[Affect measures]] |
|||
* [[Affective Computing]] |
|||
* [[Affective forecasting]] |
|||
* [[Affective neuroscience]] |
|||
* [[Affective science]] |
|||
* [[Contrasting and categorization of emotions]] |
|||
* [[CyberEmotions]] |
|||
* [[Emoticons]] |
|||
* [[Emotion classification]] |
|||
* [[Emotion in animals]] |
|||
* [[Emotions and culture]] |
|||
* [[Emotion and memory]] |
|||
* [[Emotional expression]] |
|||
* [[Emotions in virtual communication]] |
|||
* [[Empathy]] |
|||
* [[Endocrinology]] |
|||
* [[Fear]] |
|||
* [[Feeling]] |
|||
* [[Fuzzy-trace theory]] |
|||
* [[Group emotion]] |
|||
* [[International Affective Picture System]] |
|||
* [[Contrasting and categorization of emotions|List of emotions]] |
|||
* [[Affective science#Measuring emotions|Measuring Emotions]] |
|||
* [[Neuroendocrinology]] |
|||
* [[Sociology of emotions]] |
|||
* [[Social emotion]] |
|||
* [[Social neuroscience]] |
|||
* [[Social sharing of emotions]] |
|||
* [[Somatic markers hypothesis]] |
|||
{{colend}} |
|||
==References== |
|||
{{Refimprove|date=February 2012}} |
|||
'''Notes''' |
|||
{{reflist|30em}} |
|||
'''Bibliography''' |
|||
{{refbegin}} |
|||
* {{cite book |last=Denton |first=Derek |title=The Primordial Emotions: The Dawning of Consciousness |year=2006 |publisher=Oxford University Press |isbn=978-0-19-920314-7 |ref=harv}} |
|||
* {{cite book |last=Fox |first=Elaine |title=Emotion Science: An Integration of Cognitive and Neuroscientific Approaches |year=2008 |publisher=Palgrave MacMillan |isbn=978-0-230-00517-4 |ref=harv}} |
|||
{{refend}} |
|||
==Further reading== |
|||
* Dana Sugu & Amita Chaterjee [https://sites.google.com/site/sugudana/articles-1 "Flashback: Reshuffling Emotions"], ''International Journal on Humanistic Ideology'', Vol. 3 No. 1, Spring–Summer 2010. |
|||
* Cornelius, R. (1996). ''The science of emotion''. New Jersey: [[Prentice Hall]]. |
|||
* Freitas-Magalhães, A. (Ed.). (2009). Emotional Expression: The Brain and The Face. Porto: University Fernando Pessoa Press. ISBN 978-989-643-034-4. |
|||
* Freitas-Magalhães, A. (2007). ''The Psychology of Emotions: The Allure of Human Face''. Oporto: [[University Fernando Pessoa]] Press. |
|||
* González, Ana Marta (2012). The Emotions and Cultural Analysis. Burlington, VT : Ashgate. ISBN 978-1-4094-5317-8 |
|||
* Ekman, P. (1999). "[http://www.paulekman.com/wp-content/uploads/2009/02/Basic-Emotions.pdf Basic Emotions]{{dead link|date=February 2014}}". In: T. Dalgleish and M. Power (Eds.). ''Handbook of Cognition and Emotion''. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Sussex, UK:. |
|||
* [[Nico Frijda|Frijda, N.H.]] (1986). ''The Emotions''. Maison des Sciences de l'Homme and [http://www.cambridge.org/uk/catalogue/catalogue.asp?isbn=0521316006 Cambridge University Press] |
|||
* Hochschild, A.R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feelings. Berkeley: [[University of California Press]]. |
|||
* Hogan, Patrick Colm. (2011). [http://www.themontrealreview.com/2009/What-literature-teaches-us-about-emotion.php ''What Literature Teaches Us about Emotion''] Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
|||
* Hordern, Joshua. (2013). [http://ukcatalogue.oup.com/product/academic/philosophy/social/9780199646814.do#.UZuheMqmVuR ''Political Affections: Civic Participation and Moral Theology'']. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0199646813 |
|||
* LeDoux, J.E. (1986). The neurobiology of emotion. Chap. 15 in J.E. LeDoux & W. Hirst (Eds.) ''Mind and Brain: dialogues in cognitive neuroscience''. New York: Cambridge. |
|||
* Mandler, G. (1984). ''Mind and Body: Psychology of emotion and stress''. New York: Norton. |
|||
* Nussbaum, Martha C. (2001) ''Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
|||
* Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), ''Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: Vol. 1. Theories of emotion'' (pp. 3–33). New York: Academic. |
|||
* [http://www.libertary.com/book/emotion-seduction-intimacy Ridley-Duff, R.J. (2010). Emotion, Seduction and Intimacy: Alternative Perspectives on Human Behaviour (Third Edition), Seattle: Libertary Editions] |
|||
* Roberts, Robert. (2003). ''Emotions: An Essay in Aid of Moral Psychology.'' Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. |
|||
* Scherer, K. (2005). [http://www.affective-sciences.org/system/files/2005_Scherer_SSI.pdf What are emotions and how can they be measured?]{{dead link|date=February 2014}} ''Social Science Information'' Vol. 44, No. 4: 695–729. |
|||
* Solomon, R. (1993). ''The Passions: Emotions and the Meaning of Life''. Indianapolis: [[Hackett Publishing]]. |
|||
* Zeki, S. & Romaya, J.P. (2008), "Neural correlates of hate", PloS one, vol. 3, no. 10, pp. 3556. |
|||
* [http://en.wikibooks.org/wiki/Cognitive_Psychology_and_Cognitive_Neuroscience/Motivation_and_Emotion Wikibook Cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience] |
|||
* [[Dror Green]] (2011). "Emotional Training, the art of creating a sense of a safe place in a changing world". Bulgaria: Books, Publishers and the Institute of Emotional Training. |
|||
* Goldie, Peter. (2007). "Emotion". Philosophy Compass, vol. 1, issue 6 |
|||
==External links== |
|||
{{Wikiquote}} |
|||
{{wikiversity}} |
|||
{{Wiktionary}} |
|||
{{commons category|Emotions}} |
|||
* [http://www.wcl.ece.upatras.gr/ai/resources/demo-emotion-recognition-from-speech Online Demo: Emotion recognition from speech, University of Patras, Wire Communication Lab] |
|||
* [http://feelab.ufp.pt/index.swf Facial Emotion Expression Lab] |
|||
* [http://www.cnx.org/content/m14358/latest CNX.ORG: The Psychology of Emotions, Feelings and Thoughts (free online book)] |
|||
* [http://www.qmul.ac.uk/emotions/ Queen Mary Centre for the History of the Emotions] |
|||
* [http://emotion-research.net/ Humaine Emotion-Research.net: The Humaine Portal: Research on Emotions and Human-Machine Interaction] |
|||
* [http://www.philosophyofmind.net/ PhilosophyofMind.net: Philosophy of Emotions portal] |
|||
* [http://www.affective-sciences.org/ Swiss Center for Affective Sciences] |
|||
* [http://www.iep.utm.edu/e/emotion.htm The Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Theories of Emotion] |
|||
* [http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/emotion/ The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Emotion] |
|||
* [http://emotion.nsma.arizona.edu/emotion.html University of Arizona: Salk Institute:cCC] |
|||
* [http://www.mpib-berlin.mpg.de/en/research/history-of-emotions Center for the History of Emotions, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin] |
|||
{{Library resources box |
|||
|by=no |
|||
|onlinebooks=no |
|||
|others=no |
|||
|about=yes |
|||
|label=Emotions}} |
|||
{{emotion-footer}} |
|||
{{Psychology}} |
|||
[[Category:Emotion| ]] |
|||
[[Category:Limbic system]] |
|||
[[Category:Concepts]] |
|||
[[Category:Mental processes]] |
Revision as of 20:13, 18 January 2015
Part of a series on |
Emotions |
---|
In psychology and philosophy, emotion is a subjective, conscious experience characterized primarily by psychophysiological expressions, biological reactions, and mental states. A similar multicomponential description of emotion is found in sociology. For example, Peggy Thoits[1] described emotions as involving physiological components, cultural or emotional labels (e.g., anger, surprise etc.), expressive body actions, and the appraisal of situations and contexts. Emotion is often associated and considered reciprocally influential with mood, temperament, personality, disposition, and motivation.[2] It also is influenced by hormones and neurotransmitters such as dopamine, noradrenaline, serotonin, oxytocin, cortisol and GABA. Emotion is often the driving force behind motivation, positive or negative.[3] An alternative definition of emotion is a "positive or negative experience that is associated with a particular pattern of physiological activity."[4]
The physiology of emotion is closely linked to arousal of the nervous system with various states and strengths of arousal relating, apparently, to particular emotions. Emotions are a complex state of feeling that results in physical and psychological changes that influence our behaviour. Those acting primarily on emotion may seem as if they are not thinking, but cognition is an important aspect of emotion, particularly the interpretation of events. For example, the experience of fear usually occurs in response to a threat. The cognition of danger and subsequent arousal of the nervous system (e.g. rapid heartbeat and breathing, sweating, muscle tension) is an integral component to the subsequent interpretation and labeling of that arousal as an emotional state. Emotion is also linked to behavioral tendency. Extroverted people are more likely to be social and express their emotions, while introverted people are more likely to be more socially withdrawn and conceal their emotions.
Research on emotion has increased significantly over the past two decades with many fields contributing including psychology, neuroscience, endocrinology, medicine, history, sociology, and even computer science. The numerous theories that attempt to explain the origin, neurobiology, experience, and function of emotions have only fostered more intense research on this topic. Current areas of research in the concept of emotion include the development of materials that stimulate and elicit emotion. In addition PET scans and fMRI scans help study the affective processes in the brain.[5]
Etymology, definitions, and differentiation
The word "emotion" dates back to 1579, when it was adapted from the French word émouvoir, which means "to stir up". However, the earliest precursors of the word likely dates back to the very origins of language.[6] Emotions are intense feelings that are directed at someone or something.[7]
Emotions have been described as discrete and consistent responses to internal or external events which have a particular significance for the organism. Emotions are brief in duration and consist of a coordinated set of responses, which may include verbal, physiological, behavioural, and neural mechanisms.[8] Psychotherapist Michael C. Graham describes all emotions as existing on a continuum of intensity.[9] Thus fear might range from mild concern to terror or shame might range from simple embarrassment to toxic shame.[10] Emotions have also been described as biologically given and a result of evolution because they provided good solutions to ancient and recurring problems that faced our ancestors.[11] Moods are feelings that tend to be less intense than emotions and that often lack a contextual stimulus.[7]
Emotion can be differentiated from a number of similar constructs within the field of affective neuroscience:[8]
- Feelings are best understood as a subjective representation of emotions, private to the individual experiencing them.
- Moods are diffuse affective states that generally last for much longer durations than emotions and are also usually less intense than emotions.
- Affect is an encompassing term, used to describe the topics of emotion, feelings, and moods together, even though it is commonly used interchangeably with emotion.
In addition, relationships exist between emotions, such as having positive or negative influences, with direct opposites existing. These concepts are described in contrasting and categorization of emotions. Graham differentiates emotions as functional or dysfunctional and argues all functional emotions have benefits.[12]
Components
In Scherer's components processing model of emotion,[13] five crucial elements of emotion are said to exist. From the component processing perspective, emotion experience is said to require that all of these processes become coordinated and synchronized for a short period of time, driven by appraisal processes. Although the inclusion of cognitive appraisal as one of the elements is slightly controversial, since some theorists make the assumption that emotion and cognition are separate but interacting systems, the component processing model provides a sequence of events that effectively describes the coordination involved during an emotional episode.
- Cognitive appraisal: provides an evaluation of events and objects
- Bodily symptoms: the physiological component of emotional experience
- Action tendencies: a motivational component for the preparation and direction of motor responses.
- Expression: facial and vocal expression almost always accompanies an emotional state to communicate reaction and intention of actions
- Feelings: the subjective experience of emotional state once it has occurred
Classification
A distinction can be made between emotional episodes and emotional dispositions. Emotional dispositions are also comparable to character traits, where someone may be said to be generally disposed to experience certain emotions. For example, an irritable person is generally disposed to feel irritation more easily or quickly than others do. Finally, some theorists place emotions within a more general category of "affective states" where affective states can also include emotion-related phenomena such as pleasure and pain, motivational states (for example, hunger or curiosity), moods, dispositions and traits.[14]
The classification of emotions has mainly been researched from two fundamental viewpoints. The first viewpoint is that emotions are discrete and fundamentally different constructs while the second viewpoint asserts that emotions can be characterized on a dimensional basis in groupings.
Basic emotions
For more than 40 years, Paul Ekman has supported the view that emotions are discrete, measurable, and physiologically distinct. Ekman's most influential work revolved around the finding that certain emotions appeared to be universally recognized, even in cultures that were preliterate and could not have learned associations for facial expressions through media. Another classic study found that when participants contorted their facial muscles into distinct facial expressions (e.g. disgust), they reported subjective and physiological experiences that matched the distinct facial expressions. His research findings led him to classify six emotions as basic: anger, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness and surprise.[15]
Robert Plutchik agreed with Ekman's biologically driven perspective but developed the "wheel of emotions", suggesting eight primary emotions grouped on a positive or negative basis: joy versus sadness; anger versus fear; trust versus distrust; and surprise versus anticipation.[15] Some basic emotions can be modified to form complex emotions. The complex emotions could arise from cultural conditioning or association combined with the basic emotions. Alternatively, similar to the way primary colors combine, primary emotions could blend to form the full spectrum of human emotional experience. For example, interpersonal anger and disgust could blend to form contempt. Relationships exist between basic emotions, resulting in positive or negative influences.[16]
Multi-dimensional analysis
Through the use of multidimensional scaling, psychologists can map out similar emotional experiences, which allows a visual depiction of the "emotional distance" between experiences. A further step can be taken by looking at the map's dimensions of the emotional experiences. The emotional experiences are divided into two dimensions known as valences (how negative or positive the experience was) and arousal (extent of reaction to stimuli). These two dimensions can be depicted on a 2D coordinate map.[17]
Theories on the experience
Ancient Greece and Middle Ages
Theories about emotions stretch back to at least as far as the stoics of Ancient Greece and Ancient China. In the latter it was believed that excess emotion caused damage to qi, which in turn, damages the vital organs.[18] The four humours theory made popular by Hippocrates contributed to the study of emotion in the same way that it did for medicine.
Western philosophy regarded emotion in varying ways. In stoic theories it was seen as a hindrance to reason and therefore a hindrance to virtue. Aristotle believed that emotions were an essential component to virtue.[19] In the Aristotelian view all emotions (called passions) corresponded to an appetite or capacity. During the Middle Ages, the Aristotelian view was adopted and further developed by scholasticism and Thomas Aquinas[20] in particular. There are also theories in the works of philosophers such as René Descartes, Niccolò Machiavelli, Baruch Spinoza[21] and David Hume. In the 19th century emotions were considered adaptive and were studied more frequently from an empiricist psychiatric perspective.
Evolutionary theories
- 19th Century
Perspectives on emotions from evolutionary theory were initiated in the late 19th century with Charles Darwin's book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals.[22] Darwin argued that emotions actually served a purpose for humans, in communication and also in aiding their survival. Darwin, therefore, argued that emotions evolved via natural selection and therefore have universal cross-cultural counterparts. Darwin also detailed the virtues of experiencing emotions and the parallel experiences that occur in animals (see emotion in animals). This led the way for animal research on emotions and the eventual determination of the neural underpinnings of emotion.
- Contemporary
More contemporary views along the evolutionary psychology spectrum posit that both basic emotions and social emotions evolved to motivate (social) behaviors that were adaptive in the ancestral environment.[3] Current research[citation needed] suggests that emotion is an essential part of any human decision-making and planning, and the famous distinction made between reason and emotion is not as clear as it seems. Paul D. MacLean claims that emotion competes with even more instinctive responses, on one hand, and the more abstract reasoning, on the other hand. The increased potential in neuroimaging has also allowed investigation into evolutionarily ancient parts of the brain. Important neurological advances were derived from these perspectives in the 1990s by Joseph E. LeDoux and António Damásio.
Research on social emotion also focuses on the physical displays of emotion including body language of animals and humans (see affect display). For example, spite seems to work against the individual but it can establish an individual's reputation as someone to be feared.[3] Shame and pride can motivate behaviors that help one maintain one's standing in a community, and self-esteem is one's estimate of one's status.[3][23]
Somatic theories
Somatic theories of emotion claim that bodily responses, rather than cognitive interpretations, are essential to emotions. The first modern version of such theories came from William James in the 1880s. The theory lost favor in the 20th century, but has regained popularity more recently due largely to theorists such as John Cacioppo,[24] António Damásio,[25] Joseph E. LeDoux[26] and Robert Zajonc[27] who are able to appeal to neurological evidence.[citation needed]
James–Lange theory
In his 1884 article[28] William James argued that feelings and emotions were secondary to physiological phenomena. In his theory, James proposed that the perception of what he called an "exciting fact" led directly to a physiological response, known as "emotion." To account for different types of emotional experiences, James proposed that stimuli trigger activity in the autonomic nervous system, which in turn produces an emotional experience in the brain. The Danish psychologist Carl Lange also proposed a similar theory at around the same time, and therefore this theory became known as the James–Lange theory. As James wrote, "the perception of bodily changes, as they occur, is the emotion." James further claims that "we feel sad because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble, and neither we cry, strike, nor tremble because we are sorry, angry, or fearful, as the case may be."[28]
An example of this theory in action would be as follows: An emotion-evoking stimulus (snake) triggers a pattern of physiological response (increased heart rate, faster breathing, etc.), which is interpreted as a particular emotion (fear). This theory is supported by experiments in which by manipulating the bodily state induces a desired emotional state.[29] Some people may believe that emotions give rise to emotion-specific actions: e.g. "I'm crying because I'm sad," or "I ran away because I was scared." The issue with the James–Lange theory is that of causation (bodily states causing emotions and being a priori), not that of the bodily influences on emotional experience (which can be argued and is still quite prevalent today in biofeedback studies and embodiment theory).[30]
Although mostly abandoned in its original form, Tim Dalgleish argues that most contemporary neuroscientists have embraced the components of the James-Lange theory of emotions.[31]
The James–Lange theory has remained influential. Its main contribution is the emphasis it places on the embodiment of emotions, especially the argument that changes in the bodily concomitants of emotions can alter their experienced intensity. Most contemporary neuroscientists would endorse a modified James–Lange view in which bodily feedback modulates the experience of emotion." (p. 583)
Cannon–Bard theory
Walter Bradford Cannon agreed that physiological responses played a crucial role in emotions, but did not believe that physiological responses alone could explain subjective emotional experiences. He argued that physiological responses were too slow and often imperceptible and this could not account for the relatively rapid and intense subjective awareness of emotion. He also believed that the richness, variety, and temporal course of emotional experiences could not stem from physiological reactions, that reflected fairly undifferentiated fight or flight responses.[32][33] An example of this theory in action is as follows: An emotion-evoking event (snake) triggers simultaneously both a physiological response and a conscious experience of an emotion.
Phillip Bard contributed to the theory with his work on animals. Bard found that sensory, motor, and physiological information all had to pass through the diencephalon (particularly the thalamus), before being subjected to any further processing. Therefore, Cannon also argued that it was not anatomically possible for sensory events to trigger a physiological response prior to triggering conscious awareness and emotional stimuli had to trigger both physiological and experiential aspects of emotion simultaneously.[32]
Two-factor theory
Stanley Schachter formulated his theory on the earlier work of a Spanish physician, Gregorio Maranon, who injected patients with epinephrine and subsequently asked them how they felt. Interestingly, Maranon found that most of these patients felt something but in the absence of an actual emotion-evoking stimulus, the patients were unable to interpret their physiological arousal as an experienced emotion. Schachter did agree that physiological reactions played a big role in emotions. He suggested that physiological reactions contributed to emotional experience by facilitating a focused cognitive appraisal of a given physiologically arousing event and that this appraisal was what defined the subjective emotional experience. Emotions were thus a result of two-stage process: general physiological arousal, and experience of emotion. For example, the physiological arousal, heart pounding, in a response to an evoking stimulus, the sight of a bear in the kitchen. The brain then quickly scans the area, to explain the pounding, and notices the bear. Consequently, the brain interprets the pounding heart as being the result of fearing the bear.[34] With his student, Jerome Singer, Schachter demonstrated that subjects can have different emotional reactions despite being placed into the same physiological state with an injection of epinephrine. Subjects were observed to express either anger or amusement depending on whether another person in the situation (a confederate) displayed that emotion. Hence, the combination of the appraisal of the situation (cognitive) and the participants' reception of adrenaline or a placebo together determined the response. This experiment has been criticized in Jesse Prinz's (2004) Gut Reactions.
Cognitive theories
With the two-factor theory now incorporating cognition, several theories began to argue that cognitive activity in the form of judgments, evaluations, or thoughts were entirely necessary for an emotion to occur. One of the main proponents of this view was Richard Lazarus who argued that emotions must have some cognitive intentionality. The cognitive activity involved in the interpretation of an emotional context may be conscious or unconscious and may or may not take the form of conceptual processing.
Lazarus' theory is very influential; emotion is a disturbance that occurs in the following order:
- Cognitive appraisal—The individual assesses the event cognitively, which cues the emotion.
- Physiological changes—The cognitive reaction starts biological changes such as increased heart rate or pituitary adrenal response.
- Action—The individual feels the emotion and chooses how to react.
For example: Jenny sees a snake.
- Jenny cognitively assesses the snake in her presence. Cognition allows her to understand it as a danger.
- Her brain activates Adrenaline gland which pumps Adrenaline through her blood stream resulting in increased heartbeat.
- Jenny screams and runs away.
Lazarus stressed that the quality and intensity of emotions are controlled through cognitive processes. These processes underline coping strategies that form the emotional reaction by altering the relationship between the person and the environment.
George Mandler provided an extensive theoretical and empirical discussion of emotion as influenced by cognition, consciousness, and the autonomic nervous system in two books (Mind and Emotion, 1975, and Mind and Body: Psychology of Emotion and Stress, 1984)
There are some theories on emotions arguing that cognitive activity in the form of judgements, evaluations, or thoughts are necessary in order for an emotion to occur. A prominent philosophical exponent is Robert C. Solomon (for example, The Passions, Emotions and the Meaning of Life, 1993). Solomon claims that emotions are judgements. He has put forward a more nuanced view which responds to what he has called the ‘standard objection’ to cognitivism, the idea that a judgement that something is fearsome can occur with or without emotion, so judgement cannot be identified with emotion. The theory proposed by Nico Frijda where appraisal leads to action tendencies is another example.
It has also been suggested that emotions (affect heuristics, feelings and gut-feeling reactions) are often used as shortcuts to process information and influence behavior.[35] The affect infusion model (AIM) is a theoretical model developed by Joseph Forgas in the early 1990s that attempts to explain how emotion and mood interact with one's ability to process information.
- Perceptual theory
Theories dealing with perception either use one or multiples perceptions in order to find an emotion (Goldie, 2007).A recent hybrid of the somatic and cognitive theories of emotion is the perceptual theory. This theory is neo-Jamesian in arguing that bodily responses are central to emotions, yet it emphasizes the meaningfulness of emotions or the idea that emotions are about something, as is recognized by cognitive theories. The novel claim of this theory is that conceptually-based cognition is unnecessary for such meaning. Rather the bodily changes themselves perceive the meaningful content of the emotion because of being causally triggered by certain situations. In this respect, emotions are held to be analogous to faculties such as vision or touch, which provide information about the relation between the subject and the world in various ways. A sophisticated defense of this view is found in philosopher Jesse Prinz's book Gut Reactions, and psychologist James Laird's book Feelings.
- Affective events theory
This is a communication-based theory developed by Howard M. Weiss and Russell Cropanzano (1996), that looks at the causes, structures, and consequences of emotional experience (especially in work contexts). This theory suggests that emotions are influenced and caused by events which in turn influence attitudes and behaviors. This theoretical frame also emphasizes time in that human beings experience what they call emotion episodes— a "series of emotional states extended over time and organized around an underlying theme." This theory has been utilized by numerous researchers to better understand emotion from a communicative lens, and was reviewed further by Howard M. Weiss and Daniel J. Beal in their article, "Reflections on Affective Events Theory", published in Research on Emotion in Organizations in 2005.
Situated perspective on emotion
A situated perspective on emotion, developed by Paul E. Griffiths and Andrea Scarantino , emphasizes the importance of external factors in the development and communication of emotion, drawing upon the situationism approach in psychology.[36] This theory is markedly different from both cognitivist and neo-Jamesian theories of emotion, both of which see emotion as a purely internal process, with the environment only acting as a stimulus to the emotion. In contrast, a situationist perspective on emotion views emotion as the product of an organism investigating its environment, and observing the responses of other organisms. Emotion stimulates the evolution of social relationships, acting as a signal to mediate the behavior of other organisms. In some contexts, the expression of emotion (both voluntary and involuntary) could be seen as strategic moves in the transactions between different organisms. The situated perspective on emotion states that conceptual thought is not an inherent part of emotion, since emotion is an action-oriented form of skillful engagement with the world. Griffiths and Scarantino suggested that this perspective on emotion could be helpful in understanding phobias, as well as the emotions of infants and animals.
Genetics
Emotions can motivate social interactions and relationships and therefore are directly related with basic physiology, particularly with the stress systems. This is important because emotions are related to the anti-stress complex, with an oxytocin-attachment system, which plays a major role in bonding. Emotional phenotype temperaments affect social connectedness and fitness in complex social systems (Kurt Kortschal 2013). These characteristics are shared with other species and taxa and are due to the effects of genes and their continuous transmission. Information that is encoded in the DNA sequences provides the blueprint for assembling proteins that make up our cells. Zygotes require genetic information from their parental germ cells, and at every speciation event, heritable traits that have enabled its’ ancestor to survive and reproduce successfully are passed down along with new traits that could be potentially beneficial to the offspring. In the five million years since the linages leading to modern humans and chimpanzees split, only about 1.2% of their genetic material has been modified. This suggests that everything that separates us from chimpanzees must be encoded in that very small amount of DNA, including our behaviors. Students that study animal behaviors have only identified intraspecific examples of gene-dependent behavioral phenotypes. In voles (Microtus spp.) minor genetic differences have been identified in a vasopressin receptor gene that corresponds to major species differences in social organization and the mating system (Hammock & Young 2005). Another potential example with behavioral differences is the FOCP2 gene, which is involved in neural circuitry handling speech and language (Vargha-Khadem et al. 2005). Its present form in humans differed from that of the chimpanzees by only a few mutations and has been present for about 200,000 years, coinciding with the beginning of modern humans (Enard et al. 2002). Speech, language, and social organization are all part of the basis for emotions.
Neurocircuitry
Based on discoveries made through neural mapping of the limbic system, the neurobiological explanation of human emotion is that emotion is a pleasant or unpleasant mental state organized in the limbic system of the mammalian brain. If distinguished from reactive responses of reptiles, emotions would then be mammalian elaborations of general vertebrate arousal patterns, in which neurochemicals (for example, dopamine, noradrenaline, and serotonin) step-up or step-down the brain's activity level, as visible in body movements, gestures, and postures. Emotions can likely be mediated by pheromones (see fear).[37]
For example, the emotion of love is proposed to be the expression of paleocircuits of the mammalian brain (specifically, modules of the cingulate gyrus) which facilitate the care, feeding, and grooming of offspring. Paleocircuits are neural platforms for bodily expression configured before the advent of cortical circuits for speech. They consist of pre-configured pathways or networks of nerve cells in the forebrain, brain stem and spinal cord.
The motor centers of reptiles react to sensory cues of vision, sound, touch, chemical, gravity, and motion with pre-set body movements and programmed postures. With the arrival of night-active mammals, smell replaced vision as the dominant sense, and a different way of responding arose from the olfactory sense, which is proposed to have developed into mammalian emotion and emotional memory. The mammalian brain invested heavily in olfaction to succeed at night as reptiles slept—one explanation for why olfactory lobes in mammalian brains are proportionally larger than in the reptiles. These odor pathways gradually formed the neural blueprint for what was later to become our limbic brain.[37]
Emotions are thought to be related to certain activities in brain areas that direct our attention, motivate our behavior, and determine the significance of what is going on around us. Pioneering work by Broca (1878), Papez (1937), and MacLean (1952) suggested that emotion is related to a group of structures in the center of the brain called the limbic system, which includes the hypothalamus, cingulate cortex, hippocampi, and other structures. More recent research has shown that some of these limbic structures are not as directly related to emotion as others are, while some non-limbic structures have been found to be of greater emotional relevance.
In 2011, Lövheim proposed a direct relation between specific combinations of the levels of the signal substances dopamine, noradrenaline and serotonin and eight basic emotions. A model was presented where the signal substances form the axes of a coordinate system, and the eight basic emotions according to Silvan Tomkins are placed in the eight corners. Anger is, according to the model, for example produced by the combination of low serotonin, high dopamine and high noradrenaline.[38]
Prefrontal cortex
There is ample evidence that the left prefrontal cortex is activated by stimuli that cause positive approach.[39] If attractive stimuli can selectively activate a region of the brain, then logically the converse should hold, that selective activation of that region of the brain should cause a stimulus to be judged more positively. This was demonstrated for moderately attractive visual stimuli[40] and replicated and extended to include negative stimuli.[41]
Two neurobiological models of emotion in the prefrontal cortex made opposing predictions. The Valence Model predicted that anger, a negative emotion, would activate the right prefrontal cortex. The Direction Model predicted that anger, an approach emotion, would activate the left prefrontal cortex. The second model was supported.[42]
This still left open the question of whether the opposite of approach in the prefrontal cortex is better described as moving away (Direction Model), as unmoving but with strength and resistance (Movement Model), or as unmoving with passive yielding (Action Tendency Model). Support for the Action Tendency Model (passivity related to right prefrontal activity) comes from research on shyness[43] and research on behavioral inhibition.[44] Research that tested the competing hypotheses generated by all four models also supported the Action Tendency Model.[45][46]
Homeostatic/primordial emotion
Another neurological approach distinguishes two classes of emotion: "classical" emotions such as love, anger and fear that are evoked by environmental stimuli, and "primordial" or "homeostatic emotions" – attention-demanding feelings evoked by body states, such as pain, hunger and fatigue, that motivate behavior (withdrawal, eating or resting in these examples) aimed at maintaining the body's internal milieu at its ideal state.[47]
Derek Denton defines the latter as "the subjective element of the instincts, which are the genetically programmed behaviour patterns which contrive homeostasis. They include thirst, hunger for air, hunger for food, pain and hunger for specific minerals etc. There are two constituents of a primordial emotion--the specific sensation which when severe may be imperious, and the compelling intention for gratification by a consummatory act."[48]
Disciplinary approaches
Many different disciplines have produced work on the emotions. Human sciences study the role of emotions in mental processes, disorders, and neural mechanisms. In psychiatry, emotions are examined as part of the discipline's study and treatment of mental disorders in humans. Nursing studies emotions as part of its approach to the provision of holistic health care to humans. Psychology examines emotions from a scientific perspective by treating them as mental processes and behavior and they explore the underlying physiological and neurological processes. In neuroscience sub-fields such as social neuroscience and affective neuroscience, scientists study the neural mechanisms of emotion by combining neuroscience with the psychological study of personality, emotion, and mood. In linguistics, the expression of emotion may change to the meaning of sounds. In education, the role of emotions in relation to learning is examined.
Social sciences often examine emotion for the role that it plays in human culture and social interactions. In sociology, emotions are examined for the role they play in human society, social patterns and interactions, and culture. In anthropology, the study of humanity, scholars use ethnography to undertake contextual analyses and cross-cultural comparisons of a range of human activities. Some anthropology studies examine the role of emotions in human activities. In the field of communication sciences, critical organizational scholars have examined the role of emotions in organizations, from the perspectives of managers, employees, and even customers. A focus on emotions in organizations can be credited to Arlie Russell Hochschild's concept of emotional labor. The University of Queensland hosts EmoNet,[49] an e-mail distribution list representing a network of academics that facilitates scholarly discussion of all matters relating to the study of emotion in organizational settings. The list was established in January 1997 and has over 700 members from across the globe.
In economics, the social science that studies the production, distribution, and consumption of goods and services, emotions are analyzed in some sub-fields of microeconomics, in order to assess the role of emotions on purchase decision-making and risk perception. In criminology, a social science approach to the study of crime, scholars often draw on behavioral sciences, sociology, and psychology; emotions are examined in criminology issues such as anomie theory and studies of "toughness," aggressive behavior, and hooliganism. In law, which underpins civil obedience, politics, economics and society, evidence about people's emotions is often raised in tort law claims for compensation and in criminal law prosecutions against alleged lawbreakers (as evidence of the defendant's state of mind during trials, sentencing, and parole hearings). In political science, emotions are examined in a number of sub-fields, such as the analysis of voter decision-making.
In philosophy, emotions are studied in sub-fields such as ethics, the philosophy of art (for example, sensory–emotional values, and matters of taste and sentimentality), and the philosophy of music (see also Music and emotion). In history, scholars examine documents and other sources to interpret and analyze past activities; speculation on the emotional state of the authors of historical documents is one of the tools of interpretation. In literature and film-making, the expression of emotion is the cornerstone of genres such as drama, melodrama, and romance. In communication studies, scholars study the role that emotion plays in the dissemination of ideas and messages. Emotion is also studied in non-human animals in ethology, a branch of zoology which focuses on the scientific study of animal behavior. Ethology is a combination of laboratory and field science, with strong ties to ecology and evolution. Ethologists often study one type of behavior (for example, aggression) in a number of unrelated animals.
History
The history of emotions has become an increasingly popular topic recently, with some scholars arguing that it is an essential category of analysis, not unlike class, race, or gender. Historians, like other social scientists, assume that emotions, feelings and their expressions are regulated in different ways by both different cultures and different historical times, and constructivist school of history claims even that some sentiments and meta-emotions, for example Schadenfreude, are learnt and not only regulated by culture. Historians of emotion trace and analyse the changing norms and rules of feeling, while examining emotional regimes, codes, and lexicons from social, cultural or political history perspectives. Others focus on the history of medicine, science or psychology. What somebody can and may feel (and show) in a given situation, towards certain people or things, depends on social norms and rules. It is thus historically variable and open to change.[50] Several research centers have opened in the past few years in Germany, England, Spain,[51] Sweden and Australia.
Furthermore, research in historical trauma suggests that some traumatic emotions can be passed on from parents to offspring to second and even third generation, presented as examples of transgenerational trauma.
Sociology
A common way in which emotions are conceptualized in sociology is in terms of the multidimensional characteristics including cultural or emotional labels (e.g., anger, pride, fear, happiness), physiological changes (e.g., increased perspiration, changes in pulse rate), expressive facial and body movements (e.g., smiling, frowning, baring teeth), and appraisals of situational cues.[1] One comprehensive theory of emotional arousal in humans has been developed by Jonathan Turner (2007: 2009).[52][53] Two of the key eliciting factors for the arousal of emotions within this theory are expectations states and sanctions. When people enter a situation or encounter with certain expectations for how the encounter should unfold, they will experience different emotions depending on the extent to which expectations for Self, other and situation are met or not met. People can also provide positive or negative sanctions directed at Self or other which also trigger different emotional experiences in individuals. Turner analyzed a wide range of emotion theories across different fields of research including sociology, psychology, evolutionary science, and neuroscience. Based on this analysis, he identified four emotion that all researchers consider to founded on human neurology including assertive-anger, aversion-fear, satisfaction-happiness, and disappointment-sadness. These four categories are called primary emotions and there is some agreement amongst researchers that these primary emotions become combined to produce more elaborate and complex emotional experiences. These more elaborate emotions are called first-order elaborations in Turner's theory and they include sentiments such as pride, triumph, and awe. Emotions can also be experienced at different levels of intensity so that feelings of concern are a low-intensity variation of the primary emotion aversion-fear whereas depression is a higher intensity variant.
Attempts are frequently made to regulate emotion according to the conventions of the society and the situation based on many (sometimes conflicting) demands and expectations which originate from various entities. The emotion of anger is in many cultures discouraged in girls and women, while fear is discouraged in boys and men. Expectations attached to social roles, such as "acting as man" and not as a woman, and the accompanying "feeling rules" contribute to the differences in expression of certain emotions. Some cultures encourage or discourage happiness, sadness, or jealousy, and the free expression of the emotion of disgust is considered socially unacceptable in most cultures. Some social institutions are seen as based on certain emotion, such as love in the case of contemporary institution of marriage. In advertising, such as health campaigns and political messages, emotional appeals are commonly found. Recent examples include no-smoking health campaigns and political campaign advertising emphasizing the fear of terrorism.
Sociological attention to emotion has varied over time. Emilé Durkheim (1915/1965)[54] wrote about the collective effervescence or emotional energy that was experienced by members of totemic rituals in Australian aborigine society. He explained how the heightened state of emotional energy achieved during totemic rituals transported individuals above themselves giving them the sense that they were in the presence of a higher power, a force, that was embedded in the sacred objects that were worshipped. These feelings of exaltation, he argued, ultimately lead people to believe that there were forces that governed sacred objects.
In the 1990s, sociologists focused on different aspects of specific emotions and how these emotions were socially relevant. For Cooley (1992),[55] pride and shame were the most important emotions that drive people to take various social actions. During every encounter, he proposed that we monitor ourselves through the "looking glass" that the gestures and reactions of others provide. Depending on these reactions, we either experience pride or shame and this results in particular paths of action. Retzinger (1991)[56] conducted studies of married couples who experienced cycles of rage and shame. Drawing predominantly on Goffman and Cooley's work, Scheff (1990)[57] developed a microsociological theory of the social bond. The formation or disruption of social bonds is dependant on the emotions that people experience during interactions.
Subsequent to these developments, Randall Collins (2004)[58] formulated his interaction ritual theory by drawing on Durkheim's work on totemic rituals that was extended by Goffman (1964/2013; 1967)[59][60] into everyday focused encounters. Based on interaction ritual theory, we experience different levels or intensities of emotional energy during face-to-face interactions. Emotional energy is considered to be a feeling of confidence to take action and a boldness that one experiences when they are charged up from the collective effervescence generated during group gatherings that reach high levels of intensity.
There is a growing body of research applying the sociology of emotion to understanding the learning experiences of school students during classroom interactions with teachers and other students (e.g., Milne & Otieno, 2007;[61] Olitsky, 2007
- [62] Tobin, et al., 2013;[63] Zembylas, 2002[64]). These studies show that learning subjects like school science can be understood in terms of classroom interaction rituals that generate emotional energy and collective states of emotional arousal like emotional climate.
Apart from interaction ritual traditions of the sociology of emotion, other approaches have been classed into one of 6 other categories (Turner, 2009) including:
- evolutionary/biological theories,
- symbolic interactionist theories,
- dramaturgical theories,
- ritual theories,
- power and status theories,
- stratification theories, and
- exchange theories.
This list provides a general overview of different traditions in the sociology of emotion that sometimes conceptualise emotion in different ways and at other times in complementary ways. Many of these different approaches were synthesized by Turner (2007) in his sociological theory of human emotions in an attempt to produce one comprehensive sociological account that draws on developments from many of the above traditions.
Psychotherapy and regulation
Emotion regulation refers to the cognitive and behavioral strategies people use to influence their own emotional experience.[68] For example, a behavioral strategy in which one avoids a situation to avoid unwanted emotions (e.g., trying not to think about the situation, doing distracting activities, etc.).[69] Depending on the particular school's general emphasis on either cognitive components of emotion, physical energy discharging, or on symbolic movement and facial expression components of emotion,[70] different schools of psychotherapy approach the regulation of emotion differently. Cognitively oriented schools approach them via their cognitive components, such as rational emotive behavior therapy. Yet others approach emotions via symbolic movement and facial expression components (like in contemporary Gestalt therapy).[71]
Computer science
In the 2000s, research in computer science, engineering, psychology and neuroscience has been aimed at developing devices that recognize human affect display and model emotions.[72] In computer science, affective computing is a branch of the study and development of artificial intelligence that deals with the design of systems and devices that can recognize, interpret, and process human emotions. It is an interdisciplinary field spanning computer sciences, psychology, and cognitive science.[73] While the origins of the field may be traced as far back as to early philosophical enquiries into emotion,[28] the more modern branch of computer science originated with Rosalind Picard's 1995 paper[74] on affective computing.[75][76] Detecting emotional information begins with passive sensors which capture data about the user's physical state or behavior without interpreting the input. The data gathered is analogous to the cues humans use to perceive emotions in others. Another area within affective computing is the design of computational devices proposed to exhibit either innate emotional capabilities or that are capable of convincingly simulating emotions. Emotional speech processing recognizes the user's emotional state by analyzing speech patterns. The detection and processing of facial expression or body gestures is achieved through detectors and sensors.
Notable theorists
In the late 19th century, the most influential theorists were William James (1842–1910) and Carl Lange (1834–1900). James was an American psychologist and philosopher who wrote about educational psychology, psychology of religious experience/mysticism, and the philosophy of pragmatism. Lange was a Danish physician and psychologist. Working independently, they developed the James–Lange theory, a hypothesis on the origin and nature of emotions. The theory states that within human beings, as a response to experiences in the world, the autonomic nervous system creates physiological events such as muscular tension, a rise in heart rate, perspiration, and dryness of the mouth. Emotions, then, are feelings which come about as a result of these physiological changes, rather than being their cause.[77]
Silvan Tomkins (1911–1991) developed the Affect theory and Script theory. The Affect theory introduced the concept of basic emotions, and was based on the idea that the dominance of the emotion , which he called the affect system, was the motivating force in human life.[78]
Some of the most influential theorists on emotion from the 20th century have died in the last decade. They include Magda B. Arnold (1903–2002), an American psychologist who developed the appraisal theory of emotions;[79] Richard Lazarus (1922–2002), an American psychologist who specialized in emotion and stress, especially in relation to cognition; Herbert A. Simon (1916–2001), who included emotions into decision making and artificial intelligence; Robert Plutchik (1928–2006), an American psychologist who developed a psychoevolutionary theory of emotion;[80] Robert Zajonc (1923–2008) a Polish–American social psychologist who specialized in social and cognitive processes such as social facilitation. An American philosopher, Robert C. Solomon (1942–2007), contributed to the theories on the philosophy of emotions with books such as What Is An Emotion?: Classic and Contemporary Readings (Oxford, 2003). Peter Goldie (1946–2011) British philosopher who specializes in ethics, aesthetics, emotion, mood and character
Influential theorists who are still active include the following psychologists, neurologists, philosophers, and sociologists:
- Lisa Feldman Barrett – Social philosopher and psychologist specializing in affective science and human emotion.
- John Cacioppo – from the University of Chicago, founding father with Gary Berntson of social neuroscience.
- Randall Collins - (born 1941) American sociologist from the University of Pennsylvania developed the interaction ritual theory which includes emotional entrainment model
- António Damásio (born 1944) – Portuguese behavioral neurologist and neuroscientist who works in the US
- Richard Davidson (born 1951) – American psychologist and neuroscientist; pioneer in affective neuroscience.
- Paul Ekman (born 1934) – Psychologist specializing in the study of emotions and their relation to facial expressions
- Barbara Fredrickson – Social psychologist who specializes in emotions and positive psychology.
- Nico Frijda (born 1927) – Dutch psychologist who specializes in human emotions, especially facial expressions
- Arlie Russell Hochschild (born 1940) – American sociologist whose central contribution was in forging a link between the subcutaneous flow of emotion in social life and the larger trends set loose by modern capitalism within organizations.
- Joseph E. LeDoux (born 1949) – American neuroscientist who studies the biological underpinnings of memory and emotion, especially the mechanisms of fear
- George Mandler (born 1924) - American psychologist who wrote influential books on cognition and emotion
- Jaak Panksepp (born 1943) – Estonian-born American psychologist, psychobiologist and neuroscientist; pioneer in affective neuroscience.
- Jesse Prinz – American philosopher who specializes in emotion, moral psychology, aesthetics and consciousness
- James A. Russell – American psychologist who developed the circumplex model of affect
- Klaus Scherer (born 1943) – Swiss psychologist and director of the Swiss Center for Affective Sciences in Geneva; he specializes in the psychology of emotion
- Ronald de Sousa (born 1940) – English–Canadian philosopher who specializes in the philosophy of emotions, philosophy of mind and philosophy of biology.
- Jonathan Turner (born 1942) - American sociologist from the University of California, Riverside who is a general sociological theorist with specialty areas including the sociology of emotions, ethnic relations, social institutions, social stratification, and bio-sociology.
- Dominique Moïsi (born 1946) - Authored a book titled The Geopolitics of Emotion focusing on emotions related to globalization.
See also
- Affect measures
- Affective Computing
- Affective forecasting
- Affective neuroscience
- Affective science
- Contrasting and categorization of emotions
- CyberEmotions
- Emoticons
- Emotion classification
- Emotion in animals
- Emotions and culture
- Emotion and memory
- Emotional expression
- Emotions in virtual communication
- Empathy
- Endocrinology
- Fear
- Feeling
- Fuzzy-trace theory
- Group emotion
- International Affective Picture System
- List of emotions
- Measuring Emotions
- Neuroendocrinology
- Sociology of emotions
- Social emotion
- Social neuroscience
- Social sharing of emotions
- Somatic markers hypothesis
References
This article needs additional citations for verification. (February 2012) |
Notes
- ^ a b Thoits, P. A. (1989). The sociology of emotions. Annual Review of Sociology, 15, 317-342.
- ^ "Theories of Emotion". Psychology.about.com. 2013-09-13. Retrieved 2013-11-11.
- ^ a b c d Gaulin, Steven J. C. and Donald H. McBurney. Evolutionary Psychology. Prentice Hall. 2003. ISBN 978-0-13-111529-3, Chapter 6, p 121-142.
- ^ Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Second Edition. 41 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10010: Worth Publishers. p. 310. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location (link) - ^ Cacioppo, J.T & Gardner, W.L (1999). Emotion. "Annual Review of Psychology", 191.
- ^ Merriam-Webster (2004). The Merriam-Webster dictionary (11th ed.). Springfield, MA: Author.
- ^ a b Hume, D. Emotions and Moods. Organizational Behavior, 258-297.
- ^ a b Fox 2008, pp. 16–17.
- ^ Graham, Michael C. (2014). Facts of Life: ten issues of contentment. Outskirts Press. p. 63. ISBN 978-1-4787-2259-5.
- ^ Graham, Michael C. (2014). Facts of Life: Ten Issues of Contentment. Outskirts Press. ISBN 978-1-4787-2259-5.
- ^ Ekman, Paul (1992). "An argument for basic emotions". Cognition & Emotion. 6: 169–200. doi:10.1080/02699939208411068.
- ^ Graham, Michael C. (2014). Facts of Life: ten issues of contentment. Outskirts Press. ISBN 978-1-4787-2259-5.
- ^ Scherer, K. R. (2005). "What are emotions? And how can they be measured?". Social Science Information. 44: 693–727.
- ^ Schwarz, N. H. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, 2, 527-561.
- ^ a b Handel, Steven. "Classification of Emotions". Retrieved 30 April 2012.
- ^ Plutchik , R. (2002). Nature of emotions" American Scientist89, 349.
- ^ Schacter, Daniel L. (2011). Psychology Ed. 2. 41 Madison Avenue New York, NY 10010: Worth Publishers. ISBN 1-4292-3719-8.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location (link) - ^ Suchy, Yana (2011). Clinical neuropsychology of emotion. New York, NY: Guilford.
- ^ Aristotle. Nicomachean Ethics. Book 2. Chapter 6.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location (link) - ^ Aquinas, Thomas. Summa Theologica. Q.59, Art.2.
{{cite book}}
: CS1 maint: location (link) - ^ See for instance Antonio Damasio (2005) Looking for Spinoza.
- ^ Darwin, Charles (1872). The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals. Note: This book was originally published in 1872, but has been reprinted many times thereafter by different publishers
- ^ Wright, Robert. Moral animal.
- ^ Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Somatic responses to psychological stress: The reactivity hypothesis. Advances in psychological science, Vol. 2, pp. 87-114. East Sussex, United Kingdom: Psychology Press
- ^ Aziz-Zadeh L, Damasio A. (2008) Embodied semantics for actions: findings from functional brain imaging. J Physiol Paris. Jan-May;102(1-3):35-9
- ^ LeDoux J.E. (1996) The Emotional Brain. New York: Simon & Schuster.
- ^ McIntosh, D. N., Zajonc, R. B., Vig, P. S., & Emerick, S. W. (1997). Facial movement, breathing, temperature, and affect: Implications of the vascular theory of emotional efference. Cognition & Emotion, 11(2), 171-195.
- ^ a b c James, William. 1884. "What Is an Emotion?" Mind. 9, no. 34: 188-205.
- ^ Laird, James, Feelings: the Perception of Self, Oxford University Press
- ^ Reisenzein, R. (1995). James and the physical basis of emotion: A comment on ellsworth. Psychological review, 102(4), 757-761. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.102.4.757
- ^ Dalgleish, T. (2004). The emotional brain. Nature: Perspectives, 5, 582–89.
- ^ a b Cannon, Walter B. (1929). "Organization for Physiological Homeostasis". Physiological Review. 9 (3): 399–421.
- ^ Cannon, Walter B. (1927). "The James-Lange theory of emotion: A critical examination and an alternative theory". The American Journal of Psychology. 39: 106–124. doi:10.2307/1415404.
- ^ Psychology. Worth Publishers ). 2011.
{{cite book}}
: Unknown parameter|authors=
ignored (help) - ^ see the Heuristic–Systematic Model, or HSM, (Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989) under attitude change. Also see the index entry for "Emotion" in "Beyond Rationality: The Search for Wisdom in a Troubled Time" by Kenneth R. Hammond and in "Fooled by Randomness: The Hidden Role of Chance in Life and in the Markets" by Nassim Nicholas Taleb.
- ^ Griffiths, Paul Edmund and Scarantino, Andrea (2005) Emotions in the wild: The situated perspective on emotion.
- ^ a b Givens, David B. (1998). "Emotion". Center for Nonverbal Studies. Retrieved 7 May 2014.
{{cite web}}
: CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link) - ^ Lövheim H. A new three-dimensional model for emotions and monoamine neurotransmitters. Med Hypotheses (2011), Epub ahead of print. doi:10.1016/j.mehy.2011.11.016 PMID 22153577
- ^ Kringelbach, M.L.; O'Doherty, J.O.; Rolls, E.T.; & Andrews, C. (2003). Activation of the human orbitofrontal cortex to a liquid food stimulus is correlated with its subjective pleasantness. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1064–1071.
- ^ Drake, R.A. (1987). Effects of gaze manipulation on aesthetic judgments: Hemisphere priming of affect. Acta Psychologica, 65, 91–99.
- ^ Merckelbach, H.; & van Oppen, P. (1989). Effects of gaze manipulation on subjective evaluation of neutral and phobia-relevant stimuli: A comment on Drake's (1987) 'Effects of gaze manipulation on aesthetic judgments: Hemisphere priming of affect.' Acta Psychologica, 70, 147–151.
- ^ Harmon-Jones, E.; Vaughn-Scott, K.; Mohr, S.; Sigelman, J.; & Harmon-Jones, C. (2004). The effect of manipulated sympathy and anger on left and right frontal cortical activity. Emotion, 4, 95–101.
- ^ Schmidt, L.A. (1999). Frontal brain electrical activity in shyness and sociability. Psychological Science, 10, 316–320.
- ^ Garavan, H.; Ross, T.J.; & Stein, E.A. (1999). Right hemispheric dominance of inhibitory control: An event-related functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96, 8301–8306.
- ^ Drake, R.A.; & Myers, L.R. (2006). Visual attention, emotion, and action tendency: Feeling active or passive. Cognition and Emotion, 20, 608–622.
- ^ Wacker, J.; Chavanon, M.-L.; Leue, A.; & Stemmler, G. (2008). Is running away right? The behavioral activation–behavioral inhibition model of anterior asymmetry. Emotion, 8, 232–249.
- ^ Craig, A.D. (Bud) (2003). "Interoception: The sense of the physiological condition of the body" (PDF). Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 13 (4): 500–505. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00090-4. PMID 12965300.
{{cite journal}}
: External link in
(help)|last=
- ^ Denton DA, McKinley MJ, Farrell M, Egan GF (June 2009). "The role of primordial emotions in the evolutionary origin of consciousness". Conscious Cogn. 18 (2): 500–14. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2008.06.009. PMID 18701321.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ "EmoNet". Uq.edu.au. Retrieved 2013-11-11.
- ^ "History of Emotions | Max Planck Institute for Human Development". Mpib-berlin.mpg.de. Retrieved 2013-11-11.
- ^ "Cultura Emocional E Identidad". Unav.es. Retrieved 2013-11-11.
- ^ Turner, J. H. (2007). Human emotions: A sociological theory. London: Routledge.
- ^ Turner, J. H. (2009). The sociology of emotion: Basic Theoretical arguments. Emotion Review, 1(4), 340-354.
- ^ Durkheim, E. (1915/1912). The elementary forms of the religious life, trans. J. W. Swain. New York: Free Press.
- ^ Cooley, C. H. (1992). Human nature and the social order. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.
- ^ Retzinger, S. M. (1991). Violent emotions: Shame and rage in marital quarrels. London: SAGE.
- ^ Scheff, J. (1990). Microsociology: discourse, emotion and social structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
- ^ Collins, R. (2004). Interaction ritual chains. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
- ^ Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual. New York: Anchor Books.
- ^ Goffman, E. (1964/2013). Encounters: Two studies in the sociology of interactions. Mansfiled Centre, CT: Martino Publishing.
- ^ Milne, C., & Otieno, T. (2007). Understanding engagement: Science demonstrations and emotional energy. Science Education, 91, 532-553. DOI 10.1002/sce
- ^ Olitsky, S. (2007). Science learning, status and identity formation in an urban middle school. In W.-M. Roth & K. G. Tobin (Eds.), Science, learning, identity: Sociocultural and cultural-historical perspectives. (pp. 41-62). Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense.
- ^ Tobin, K., Ritchie, S. M., Oakley, J., Mergard, V., & Hudson, P. (2013). Relationships between emotional climate and the fluency of classroom interactions. Learning Environments Research, 16, 71-89.
- ^ Zembylas, M. (2002). Constructing genealogies of teachers’ emotions in science teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39, 79–103.
- ^ Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (2005) FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6:131–138
- ^ Enard W,KhaitovichP,KloseJ,Zo ̈llnerS,HeissigF,GiavaliscoP,Nieselt-StruweK,Much-more E, Varki A, Ravid R, Doxiadis GM, Bontrop RE, Pa ̈a ̈bo S (2002) Intra- and interspecific variation in primate gene expression patterns. Science 296:340–343
- ^ Hammock E.A.D, Young L.J. Microsatellite instability generates diversity in brain and sociobehavioral traits. Science. 2005;308:1630–1634.
- ^ Schacter, Daniel. "Psychology". Worth Publishers. 2011. p.316
- ^ Schacter, Daniel. "Psychology". Worth Publishers. 2011. p.340
- ^ Freitas-Magalhães, A., & Castro, E. (2009). Facial Expression: The effect of the smile in the Treatment of Depression. Empirical Study with Portuguese Subjects. In A. Freitas-Magalhães (Ed.), Emotional Expression: The Brain and The Face (pp. 127–140). Porto: University Fernando Pessoa Press. ISBN 978-989-643-034-4
- ^ "On Emotion – an article from Manchester Gestalt Centre website". 123webpages.co.uk. Retrieved 2013-11-11.
- ^ Fellous, Armony & LeDoux, 2002
- ^ Tao, Jianhua; Tieniu Tan (2005). "LNCS". Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction. Vol. 3784. Springer. pp. 981–995. doi:10.1007/11573548.
{{cite conference}}
: Unknown parameter|booktitle=
ignored (|book-title=
suggested) (help) - ^ "Affective Computing" MIT Technical Report #321 (Abstract), 1995
- ^ Kleine-Cosack, Christian (October 2006). "Recognition and Simulation of Emotions" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on May 28, 2008. Retrieved May 13, 2008.
The introduction of emotion to computer science was done by Pickard (sic) who created the field of affective computing.
- ^ Diamond, David (December 2003). "The Love Machine; Building computers that care". Wired. Retrieved May 13, 2008.
Rosalind Picard, a genial MIT professor, is the field's godmother; her 1997 book, Affective Computing, triggered an explosion of interest in the emotional side of computers and their users.
- ^ Cherry, Kendra. "v". Retrieved 30 April 2012.
- ^ The Tomkins Institute. "Applied Studies in Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition". Retrieved 30 April 2012.
- ^ Reisenzein , R. (2006). Cognition & emotion. Psychology Press, part of the Taylor & Francis Group, 20(7), 920-951. doi:10.1080/02699930600616445
- ^ Plutchik, R. (1982). A psychoevolutionary theory of emotions. Social Science Information, 21, 529. doi:10.1177/053901882021004003
Bibliography
- Denton, Derek (2006). The Primordial Emotions: The Dawning of Consciousness. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-920314-7.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help) - Fox, Elaine (2008). Emotion Science: An Integration of Cognitive and Neuroscientific Approaches. Palgrave MacMillan. ISBN 978-0-230-00517-4.
{{cite book}}
: Invalid|ref=harv
(help)
Further reading
- Dana Sugu & Amita Chaterjee "Flashback: Reshuffling Emotions", International Journal on Humanistic Ideology, Vol. 3 No. 1, Spring–Summer 2010.
- Cornelius, R. (1996). The science of emotion. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.
- Freitas-Magalhães, A. (Ed.). (2009). Emotional Expression: The Brain and The Face. Porto: University Fernando Pessoa Press. ISBN 978-989-643-034-4.
- Freitas-Magalhães, A. (2007). The Psychology of Emotions: The Allure of Human Face. Oporto: University Fernando Pessoa Press.
- González, Ana Marta (2012). The Emotions and Cultural Analysis. Burlington, VT : Ashgate. ISBN 978-1-4094-5317-8
- Ekman, P. (1999). "Basic Emotions[dead link]". In: T. Dalgleish and M. Power (Eds.). Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Sussex, UK:.
- Frijda, N.H. (1986). The Emotions. Maison des Sciences de l'Homme and Cambridge University Press
- Hochschild, A.R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feelings. Berkeley: University of California Press.
- Hogan, Patrick Colm. (2011). What Literature Teaches Us about Emotion Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Hordern, Joshua. (2013). Political Affections: Civic Participation and Moral Theology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0199646813
- LeDoux, J.E. (1986). The neurobiology of emotion. Chap. 15 in J.E. LeDoux & W. Hirst (Eds.) Mind and Brain: dialogues in cognitive neuroscience. New York: Cambridge.
- Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and Body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.
- Nussbaum, Martha C. (2001) Upheavals of Thought: The Intelligence of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: Vol. 1. Theories of emotion (pp. 3–33). New York: Academic.
- Ridley-Duff, R.J. (2010). Emotion, Seduction and Intimacy: Alternative Perspectives on Human Behaviour (Third Edition), Seattle: Libertary Editions
- Roberts, Robert. (2003). Emotions: An Essay in Aid of Moral Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- Scherer, K. (2005). What are emotions and how can they be measured?[dead link] Social Science Information Vol. 44, No. 4: 695–729.
- Solomon, R. (1993). The Passions: Emotions and the Meaning of Life. Indianapolis: Hackett Publishing.
- Zeki, S. & Romaya, J.P. (2008), "Neural correlates of hate", PloS one, vol. 3, no. 10, pp. 3556.
- Wikibook Cognitive psychology and cognitive neuroscience
- Dror Green (2011). "Emotional Training, the art of creating a sense of a safe place in a changing world". Bulgaria: Books, Publishers and the Institute of Emotional Training.
- Goldie, Peter. (2007). "Emotion". Philosophy Compass, vol. 1, issue 6
External links
- Online Demo: Emotion recognition from speech, University of Patras, Wire Communication Lab
- Facial Emotion Expression Lab
- CNX.ORG: The Psychology of Emotions, Feelings and Thoughts (free online book)
- Queen Mary Centre for the History of the Emotions
- Humaine Emotion-Research.net: The Humaine Portal: Research on Emotions and Human-Machine Interaction
- PhilosophyofMind.net: Philosophy of Emotions portal
- Swiss Center for Affective Sciences
- The Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Theories of Emotion
- The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy: Emotion
- University of Arizona: Salk Institute:cCC
- Center for the History of Emotions, Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin