Jump to content

Empathy: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
→‎Ethical issues: a bit more concise. Where she works is not important for the understanding of empathy.
→‎Emotional and cognitive empathy: added a sentence about theory of mind (edited with ProveIt)
Line 41: Line 41:
Empathy can be divided into two major components:
Empathy can be divided into two major components:
* '''Emotional empathy''', also called '''affective empathy''': the drive to respond with an appropriate emotion to another's mental states.<ref name = Rogers /> Our ability to empathize emotionally is supposed to be based on emotional contagion:<ref name=Shamay-Tsoory/> being affected by another’s emotional or arousal state.<ref name = Waal2008>{{cite journal | url=http://www.life.umd.edu/faculty/wilkinson/BIOL608W/deWaalAnnRevPsych2008.pdf | title=Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy | author=Frans B.M. deWaal | journal=Annu. Rev. Psychol. | year=2008 | issue=59 | pages=279–300 | doi=10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625}}</ref>
* '''Emotional empathy''', also called '''affective empathy''': the drive to respond with an appropriate emotion to another's mental states.<ref name = Rogers /> Our ability to empathize emotionally is supposed to be based on emotional contagion:<ref name=Shamay-Tsoory/> being affected by another’s emotional or arousal state.<ref name = Waal2008>{{cite journal | url=http://www.life.umd.edu/faculty/wilkinson/BIOL608W/deWaalAnnRevPsych2008.pdf | title=Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy | author=Frans B.M. deWaal | journal=Annu. Rev. Psychol. | year=2008 | issue=59 | pages=279–300 | doi=10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625}}</ref>
* '''Cognitive empathy''' (also termed 'theory of mind' or [[Mentalization|'mentalizing']]): the drive to identify another's mental states.<ref name = Rogers>{{cite journal | author = Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A | year = 2007 | month = Apr | title = Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome | url = | journal = J Autism Dev Disord | volume = 37 | issue = 4| pages = 709–15 }}</ref><ref name="Baron-Cohen, 2003"/>
* '''Cognitive empathy''' (also termed 'theory of mind' or [[Mentalization|'mentalizing']]): the drive to identify another's mental states.<ref name = Rogers>{{cite journal | author = Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A | year = 2007 | month = Apr | title = Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome | url = | journal = J Autism Dev Disord | volume = 37 | issue = 4| pages = 709–15 }}</ref><ref name="Baron-Cohen, 2003"/> The term cognitive empathy and [[theory of mind]] are often used synonymously.<ref>{{cite journal | url=http://www.cog.psy.ruhr-uni-bochum.de/papers/2007/Rogers%282007%29_JAutismDevDisord.pdf | title=Who Cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome | author=Kimberley Rogers, Isabel Dziobek, Jason Hassenstab, Oliver T. Wolf, Antonio Convit | journal=J Autism Dev Disord | year=2007 | issue=37 | pages=709–715 | doi=10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8}}</ref><br />
Although science has not yet agreed upon a precise definition of these constructs, there is consensus about this distinction.<ref name = Cox>{{cite journal | url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21896497 | title=The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics | author=Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. | journal=Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. | year=2012 | month=August | volume=7 | issue=6 | pages=727-37 | doi=10.1093/scan/nsr051}}</ref> A meta-analysis of recent fMRI studies of empathy confirmed that different brain areas are activated during affective–perceptual empathy and cognitive–evaluative empathy.<ref>{{cite journal | url=http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezp.sub.su.se/science/article/pii/S0149763410001739 | title=Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis | author=Yan Fan, Niall W. Duncana, Moritz de Greck, Georg Northoff | journal=Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews | year=2011 | volume=35 | issue=3 | pages=903–911 | doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009}}</ref> Also a study with patients with different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.<ref name=Shamay-Tsoory>{{cite journal | url=http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/132/3/617.full | title=Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions | author=Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Judith Aharon-Peretz and Daniella Perry | journal=Brain | year=2009 | volume=132 | issue=3 | pages=617-627 | doi=10.1093/brain/awn279}}</ref> Furthermore, there is a difference in disturbance of affective versus cognitive empathy in different psychiatric disorders.<ref name = Cox />
Although science has not yet agreed upon a precise definition of these constructs, there is consensus about this distinction.<ref name = Cox>{{cite journal | url=http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21896497 | title=The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics | author=Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. | journal=Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. | year=2012 | month=August | volume=7 | issue=6 | pages=727-37 | doi=10.1093/scan/nsr051}}</ref> A meta-analysis of recent fMRI studies of empathy confirmed that different brain areas are activated during affective–perceptual empathy and cognitive–evaluative empathy.<ref>{{cite journal | url=http://www.sciencedirect.com.ezp.sub.su.se/science/article/pii/S0149763410001739 | title=Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis | author=Yan Fan, Niall W. Duncana, Moritz de Greck, Georg Northoff | journal=Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews | year=2011 | volume=35 | issue=3 | pages=903–911 | doi=10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009}}</ref> Also a study with patients with different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.<ref name=Shamay-Tsoory>{{cite journal | url=http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/content/132/3/617.full | title=Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions | author=Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Judith Aharon-Peretz and Daniella Perry | journal=Brain | year=2009 | volume=132 | issue=3 | pages=617-627 | doi=10.1093/brain/awn279}}</ref> Furthermore, there is a difference in disturbance of affective versus cognitive empathy in different psychiatric disorders.<ref name = Cox />



Revision as of 11:14, 1 January 2013

Empathy is the capacity to recognize feelings that are being experienced by another sentient or fictional being. Someone may need to have a certain amount of empathy before they are able to feel compassion. The English word was coined in 1909 by Edward B. Titchener as an attempt to translate the German word "Einfühlungsvermögen", a new phenomenon explored at the end of 19th century mainly by Theodor Lipps. It was later re-translated into the German language (Germanized) as "Empathie", and is still in use there.[1]

Etymology

The English word is derived from the Greek word ἐμπάθεια (empatheia), "physical affection, passion, partiality" which comes from ἐν (en), "in, at" + πάθος (pathos), "passion" or "suffering".[2] The term was adapted by Hermann Lotze and Robert Vischer to create the German word Einfühlung ("feeling into"), which was translated by Edward B. Titchener into the English term empathy.[3]

Alexithymia from the Ancient Greek words λέξις (lexis) and θυμός (thumos) modified by an alpha-privative—literally "without words for emotions"—is a term to describe a state of deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing emotions in oneself.[4][5]

Note that in modern Greek the word empathy (εμπάθεια) translates as "hatred" or "spitefulness" (a situation of causing passion, rather than mutual relation to one's passion); ενσυναίσθηση is the correct modern equivalent of empathy.

Theorists and definition

Empathy is an ability with many different definitions. They cover a broad spectrum, ranging from caring for other people and having a desire to help them, to experiencing emotions that match another person's emotions, to knowing what the other person is thinking or feeling, to blurring the line between self and other.[6] Below are definitions of empathy:

  • Daniel Batson: "A motivation oriented towards the other."[7]
  • D. M. Berger: "The capacity to know emotionally what another is experiencing from within the frame of reference of that other person, the capacity to sample the feelings of another or to put one's self in another's shoes."[8]
  • Jean Decety: "A sense of similarity in feelings experienced by the self and the other, without confusion between the two individuals."[9][10]
  • Frans de Waal: "The capacity to (a) be affected by and share the emotional state of another, (b) assess the reasons for the other's state, and (c) identify with the other, adopting his or her perspective. This definition extends beyond what exists in many animals, but the term "empathy" … applies even if only criterion (a) is met."[11]
  • Nancy Eisenberg: "An affective response that stems from the apprehension or comprehension of another's emotional state or condition, and that is similar to what the other person is feeling or would be expected to feel."[12]
  • Edith Stein: "Empathy cannot be forced to occur but can be facilitated by good listening skills, self awareness, and self confidence that will demonstrate empathatic willingness by clinicians."[13]
  • Alvin Goldman: "The ability to put oneself into the mental shoes of another person to understand her emotions and feelings."[14]
  • Martin Hoffman: any process where the attended perception of the object's state generates a state in the subject that is more applicable to the object's state or situation than to the subject's own prior state or situation.[15]
  • William Ickes: A complex form of psychological inference in which observation, memory, knowledge, and reasoning are combined to yield insights into the thoughts and feelings of others.[16]
  • Heinz Kohut: Empathy is the capacity to think and feel oneself into the inner life of another person[17]
  • Harry Prosen: "an emotional understanding which allows one as a therapist to resonate with one's patients in depth emotionally, so that it influences the therapeutic approach and alliance with the patient"[18]
  • Carl Rogers: To perceive the internal frame of reference of another with accuracy and with the emotional components and meanings which pertain thereto as if one were the person, but without ever losing the "as if" condition. Thus, it means to sense the hurt or the pleasure of another as he senses it and to perceive the causes thereof as he perceives them, but without ever losing the recognition that it is as if I were hurt or pleased and so forth.[19]
  • Marshall Rosenberg: "Empathic connection is an understanding of the heart in which we see the beauty in the other person, the divine energy in the other person, the life that's alive in them."[20]
  • Roy Schafer: Empathy involves the inner experience of sharing in and comprehending the momentary psychological state of another person.[21]
  • Wynn Schwartz: We recognize others as empathic when we feel that they have accurately acted on or somehow acknowledged in stated or unstated fashion our values or motivations, our knowledge, and our skills or competence, but especially as they appear to recognize the significance of our actions in a manner that we can tolerate their being recognized.[22]
  • Edith Stein: Empathy is the experience of foreign consciousness in general.[23]
  • Simon Baron-Cohen (2003): Empathy is about spontaneously and naturally tuning into the other person's thoughts and feelings, whatever these might be.[24]
  • Khen Lampert (2005): "[Empathy] is what happens to us when we leave our own bodies...and find ourselves either momentarily or for a longer period of time in the mind of the other. We observe reality through her eyes, feel her emotions, share in her pain."[25]

Since empathy involves understanding the emotional states of other people, the way it is characterized is derivative of the way emotions themselves are characterized. If, for example, emotions are taken to be centrally characterized by bodily feelings, then grasping the bodily feelings of another will be central to empathy. On the other hand, if emotions are more centrally characterized by a combination of beliefs and desires, then grasping these beliefs and desires will be more essential to empathy. The ability to imagine oneself as another person is a sophisticated imaginative process. However, the basic capacity to recognize emotions is probably innate[26] and may be achieved unconsciously. Yet it can be trained[27] and achieved with various degrees of intensity or accuracy.

The human capacity to recognize the bodily feelings of another is related to one's imitative capacities and seems to be grounded in the innate capacity to associate the bodily movements and facial expressions one sees in another with the proprioceptive feelings of producing those corresponding movements or expressions oneself.[28] Humans seem to make the same immediate connection between the tone of voice and other vocal expressions and inner feeling.

Empathy is distinct from sympathy, pity, and emotional contagion.[29] Sympathy or empathic concern is the feeling of compassion or concern for another, the wish to see them better off or happier. Pity is feeling that another is in trouble and in need of help as they cannot fix their problems themselves, often described as "feeling sorry" for someone. Emotional contagion is when a person (especially an infant or a member of a mob) imitatively "catches" the emotions that others are showing without necessarily recognizing this is happening.[30]

Emotional and cognitive empathy

Empathy can be divided into two major components:

  • Emotional empathy, also called affective empathy: the drive to respond with an appropriate emotion to another's mental states.[31] Our ability to empathize emotionally is supposed to be based on emotional contagion:[32] being affected by another’s emotional or arousal state.[33]
  • Cognitive empathy (also termed 'theory of mind' or 'mentalizing'): the drive to identify another's mental states.[31][24] The term cognitive empathy and theory of mind are often used synonymously.[34]

Although science has not yet agreed upon a precise definition of these constructs, there is consensus about this distinction.[35] A meta-analysis of recent fMRI studies of empathy confirmed that different brain areas are activated during affective–perceptual empathy and cognitive–evaluative empathy.[36] Also a study with patients with different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.[32] Furthermore, there is a difference in disturbance of affective versus cognitive empathy in different psychiatric disorders.[35]

Emotional empathy can be subdivided into:

  • Personal distress: the inclination to experience self-centered feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to another’s suffering.[37][38]
  • Empathic concern: the inclination to experience of sympathy and compassion towards others in response to their suffering.[37][38]

There is no consensus regarding the question if personal distress is a basic form of empathy or if it falls outside of empathy.[37] There is a developmental aspect to this subdivision. Infants respond to the distress of others by getting distressed themselves; only when they are 2 years old they start to respond in other-oriented ways, trying to help, comfort and share.[37]

Development

File:Empathy Children.jpg
When children are shown videoclips with situations where they see people suffering pain by coincidence, neural circuits related to pain are being activated in their brain.

By the age of two, children normally begin to display the fundamental behaviors of empathy by having an emotional response that corresponds with another person.[39] Even earlier, at one year of age, infants have some rudiments of empathy, in the sense that they understand that, just like their own actions, other people's actions have goals.[40][41][42] Sometimes, toddlers will comfort others or show concern for them at as early an age as two. Also during the second year, toddlers will play games of falsehood or "pretend" in an effort to fool others, and this requires that the child know what others believe before he or she can manipulate those beliefs.[43]

According to researchers at the University of Chicago who used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), children between the ages of 7 and 12 appear to be naturally inclined to feel empathy for others in pain. Their findings[44] are consistent with previous fMRI studies of pain empathy with adults. The research also found additional aspects of the brain were activated when youngsters saw another person intentionally hurt by another individual, including regions involved in moral reasoning.[45]

Despite being able to show some signs of empathy, such as attempting to comfort a crying baby, from as early as 18 months to two years, most children do not show a fully fledged theory of mind until around the age of four.[46] Theory of mind involves the ability to understand that other people may have beliefs that are different from one's own, and is thought to involve the cognitive component of empathy.[24] Children usually become capable of passing "false belief" tasks, considered to be a test for a theory of mind, around the age of four. Individuals with autism often find using a theory of mind very difficult (e.g. Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith, 1988; the Sally-Anne test).

Empathetic maturity is a cognitive structural theory developed at the Yale University School of Nursing and addresses how adults conceive or understand the personhood of patients. The theory, first applied to nurses and since applied to other professions, postulates three levels that have the properties of cognitive structures. The third and highest level is held to be a meta-ethical theory of the moral structure of care. Those adults operating with level-III understanding synthesize systems of justice and care-based ethics.[47]

Neurological basis

Research in recent years has focused on possible brain processes underlying the experience of empathy. For instance, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been employed to investigate the functional anatomy of empathy.[48][49] These studies have shown that observing another person's emotional state activates parts of the neuronal network involved in processing that same state in oneself, whether it is disgust,[50] touch,[51][52] or pain.[53][54][55][56] The study of the neural underpinnings of empathy has received increased interest following the target paper published by Preston and Frans de Waal,[57] following the discovery of mirror neurons in monkeys that fire both when the creature watches another perform an action as well as when they themselves perform it. In their paper, they argued that attended perception of the object's state automatically activates neural representations, and that this activation automatically primes or generates the associated autonomic and somatic responses (idea of perception-action-coupling [58]), unless inhibited. This mechanism is similar to the common coding theory between perception and action. Another recent study provides evidence of separate neural pathways activating reciprocal suppression in different regions of the brain associated with the performance of "social" and "mechanical" tasks. These findings suggest that the cognition associated with reasoning about the "state of another person's mind" and "causal/mechanical properties of inanimate objects" are neurally suppressed from occurring at the same time.[59][60]

Anger and distress

Anger

Empathic anger is an emotion, a form of empathic distress.[61] Empathic anger is felt in a situation where someone else is being hurt by another person or thing. It is possible to see this form of anger as a pro-social emotion.

Empathic anger has direct effects on both helping and punishing desires. It can be divided to trait and state empathic angers.[62]

The relationship between empathy and anger response towards another person has also been investigated, with dispositional perspective taking found to be significantly negatively related to anger arousal (i.e. the higher a person's perspective taking ability, the less angry they were in response to a provocation). Empathic concern did not, however, significantly predict anger response, and higher personal distress was associated with increased anger.[63]

Distress

Empathic distress is feeling the perceived pain of another person. This feeling can be transformed into empathic anger, feelings of injustice, or guilt. These emotions can be perceived as pro-social, and some say they can be seen as motives for moral behavior.[61]

Atypical empathic response

Atypical empathic responses are found in people on the autism spectrum, some personality disorders such as narcissistic personality disorder and borderline personality disorder, psychopathy, schizophrenia, depersonalization, bipolar disorder[35] and conduct disorder.[64]

Autism spectrum

The interaction between empathy and the autism spectrum is a complex and ongoing field of research. Several different factors are proposed to be at play here.

Alexithymia

Research suggests that 85% of ASD individuals have alexithymia,[65] which involves not just the inability to verbally express emotions, but specifically the inability to identify emotional states in self or others.[66] According to recent fMRI studies[67] the syndrome of alexithymia, a condition in which an individual is rendered incapable of recognising and articulating emotional arousal in self or others, is responsible for a severe lack of emotional empathy.[67] The lack of empathic attunement inherent to alexithymic states may reduce quality[68] and satisfaction[69] of relationships. Recently, a study has shown that high-functioning adults with autism appear to have a range of responses to music similar to that of neurotypical individuals, including the deliberate use of music for mood management. Clinical treatment of alexithymia could involve using a simple associative learning process between musically induced emotions and their cognitive correlates.[70]

Theory of mind

Previous studies have suggested that autistic individuals have an impaired theory of mind (ToM). ToM and empathy are often used synonymously, but these capacities represent different abilities that rely on different neuronal circuitry. ToM is the ability to understand mental states such as intentions, goals and beliefs, and relies on structures of the temporal lobe and the pre-frontal cortex. However, empathy is the ability to share the feelings of others and relies on the sensorimotor cortices as well as limbic and para-limbic structures. Francesca Happe showed that autistic children who demonstrate a lack of theory of mind (cognitive empathy) lack theory of mind for self as well as for others.[71]

Mirror neuron activity

One study found that, relative to typically developing children, high-functioning children with autism showed reduced mirror neuron activity in the brain's inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) while imitating and observing emotional expressions.[72] EEG evidence revealed that there was significantly greater mu suppression in the sensorimotor cortex of autistic individuals. Activity in this area was inversely related to symptom severity in the social domain, suggesting that a dysfunctional mirror neuron system may underlie the social deficits observed in autism.[73]

Cognitive versus affective empathy

Rogers' research, following the distinction between cognitive empathy and affective empathy, suggests that people with Asperger syndrome have less ability to ascertain others' feelings (in terms of theory of mind), but demonstrate equal empathy when they are aware of others' states of mind (in terms of affect).[31]
Regarding the subdivision of emotional empathy into personal distress and empathic concern, individuals with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) self-report lower levels of empathic concern, and they show less or absent comforting responses toward someone who is suffering. However, individuals with ASD also report equal or higher levels of personal distress compared to controls. The combination of reduced empathic concern and increased personal distress may lead to the overall reduction of empathy in ASD.[37] Social psychology research found that when a person is overwhelmed by his or her own feelings when observing a person who needs help, he or she is unlikely to engage in comforting or helping others.[37]

As regards the failure of empathic response, it would appear that at least some people with autism are oversensitive to the feelings of others rather than immune to them, but cannot handle the painful feed-back that this initiates in the body, and have therefore learnt to suppress this facility.[74]

Sex differences and autism

It has been shown that males are generally less empathetic than females.[75] The "Extreme Male Brain" (EMB) theory proposes that individuals on the autistic spectrum are characterized by impairments in empathy due to sex differences in the brain: specifically, people with autism-spectrum conditions show an exaggerated male profile. A study showed that some aspects of autistic neuroanatomy seem to be extremes of typical male neuroanatomy. One of the main questions that needs to be answered is: if autism is an extreme of the male brain, then is it the result of elevated fetal testosterone (FT), abnormalities in androgen receptors or the genes controlling FT, or sexually dimorphic gene expression unrelated to FT? Future research will need to map all aspects of autistic neuroanatomy that are hypermasculinized as well as consider how to explain those aspects that are not. If the EMB theory does apply to autism, and the male brain is a risk factor for autism, this may explain the lower prevalence in females. However, another study has suggested that Alexithymia, a co-morbid condition prevalent in up to 85% of people on the Autism Spectrum, is linked to the empathy impairment instead of Autism.[76]

Personality disorders

Atypical empathy is a trait of some personality disorders, including psychopathy, narcissistic personality disorder, and schizoid personality disorder.

Psychopathy

Psychopaths can detect the emotions of others and mimic caring and friendship, possibly in an effort to exploit others, without empathizing or reciprocating these emotions. Some research indicates that components of neural circuits involved in empathy may be dysfunctional in psychopaths.[77]

Narcissistic personality disorder

One diagnostic criterion of narcissistic personality disorder is a lack of empathy and an unwillingness to recognize or identify with the feelings and needs of others.[78]

Schizoid personality disorder

Characteristics of schizoid personality disorder include emotional coldness, detachment, and impaired affect corresponding with an inability to be empathetic and sensitive towards others.[79][80][81][82]

Conduct disorder

A study conducted by Jean Decety and colleagues at the University of Chicago demonstrated that subjects with aggressive conduct disorder elicit atypical empathic responses to viewing others in pain.[64] Subjects with conduct disorder were at least as responsive as controls to the pain of others, but unlike controls, subjects with conduct disorder showed strong and specific activation of the amygdala and ventral striatum (areas that enable a general arousing effect of reward), yet impaired activation of the neural regions involved in self-regulation and metacognition (including moral reasoning), in addition to diminished processing between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.[64]

Practical issues

Proper empathic engagement helps one to understand and anticipate the behavior of another. Apart from the automatic tendency to recognise the emotions of others, one may also deliberately engage in empathic reasoning. Two general methods have been identified here [11]. A person may simulate 'pretend' versions of the beliefs, desires, character traits and context of the other and see what emotional feelings this leads to. Or, a person may simulate the emotional feeling and then look around for a suitable reason for this to fit.

Some research suggests that people are more able and willing to empathize with those most similar to themselves. In particular, empathy increases with similarities in culture and living conditions. Empathy is more likely to occur between individuals whose interaction is more frequent. (See Levenson and Reuf 1997 and Hoffman 2000: 62). A measure of how well a person can infer the specific content of another person's thoughts and feelings has been developed by William Ickes (1997, 2003). Ickes and his colleagues have developed a video-based method to measure empathic accuracy and have used this method to study the empathic inaccuracy of maritally aggressive and abusive spouses, among other topics.

There are concerns that the empathiser's own emotional background may affect or distort what emotions they perceive in others (e.g. Goleman 1996: p. 104). Empathy is not a process that is likely to deliver certain judgments about the emotional states of others. It is a skill that is gradually developed throughout life, and which improves the more contact we have with the person with whom one empathizes. Accordingly, any knowledge gained of the emotions of the other must be revisable in light of further information.

Ethical issues

The extent to which a person's emotions are publicly observable, or mutually recognized as such has significant social consequences. Empathic recognition may or may not be welcomed or socially desirable. This is particularly the case where we recognize the emotions that someone has towards us during real time interactions. Based on a metaphorical affinity with touch, philosopher Edith Wyschogrod claims that the proximity entailed by empathy increases the potential vulnerability of either party.[83] The appropriate role of empathy in our dealings with others is highly dependent on the circumstances. For instance,Tania Singer claims that clinicians or caregivers must take care not to be too sensitive to the emotions of others, to over-invest their own emotions, at the risk of draining away their own resourcefulness.[84] Furthermore an awareness of the limitations of empathic accuracy is prudent in a caregiving situation.

Disciplinary approaches

Psychotherapy

Heinz Kohut is the main introducer of the principle of empathy in psychoanalysis. His principle applies to the method of gathering unconscious material. The possibility of not applying the principle is granted in the cure, for instance when you must reckon with another principle, that of reality. Developing skills of empathy is often a central theme in the recovery process for drug addicts.[citation needed]

In evolutionary psychology, attempts at explaining pro-social behavior often mention the presence of empathy in the individual as a possible variable. Although exact motives behind complex social behaviors are difficult to distinguish, the "ability to put oneself in the shoes of another person and experience events and emotions the way that person experienced them" is the definitive factor for truly altruistic behavior according to Batson's empathy-altruism hypothesis. If empathy is not felt, social exchange (what's in it for me?) supersedes pure altruism, but if empathy is felt, an individual will help by actions or by word, regardless of whether it is in their self-interest to do so and even if the costs outweigh potential rewards.[85]

Education

An important target of the method Learning by teaching (LbT) is to train systematically and, in each lesson, teach empathy. Students have to transmit new content to their classmates, so they have to reflect continuously on the mental processes of the other students in the classroom. This way it is possible to develop step-by-step the students' feeling for group reactions and networking.

Evolution of empathy

An increasing number of studies in animal behavior and neuroscience claim that empathy is not restricted to humans, and is in fact as old as the mammals, or perhaps older. Examples include dolphins saving humans from drowning or from shark attacks, and a multitude of behaviors observed in primates, both in captivity and in the wild, and in particular in bonobos, which are reported as the most empathetic of all the primates.[86][87] A recent study has demonstrated prosocial behavior elicited by empathy in rodents.[88]

Rodents have been shown to demonstrate empathy for cagemates (but not strangers) in pain.[89] One of the most widely read studies on the evolution of empathy, which discusses a neural perception-action mechanism (PAM), is the one by Stephanie Preston and de Waal ([11]). This review postulates a bottom-up model of empathy that ties together all levels, from state matching to perspective-taking. For University of Chicago neurobiologist Jean Decety, [empathy] is not specific to humans. He argues that there is strong evidence that empathy has deep evolutionary, biochemical, and neurological underpinnings, and that even the most advanced forms of empathy in humans are built on more basic forms and remain connected to core mechanisms associated with affective communication, social attachment, and parental care.[90] Core neural circuits that are involved in empathy and caring include the brainstem, the amygdala, hypothalamus, basal ganglia, insula and orbitofrontal cortex.[91]

Fiction

Some philosophers (such as Martha Nussbaum) suggest that novel reading cultivates readers' empathy and leads them to exercise better world citizenship. For a critique of this application of the empathy-altruism hypothesis to experiences of narrative empathy, see Keen's Empathy and the Novel (Oxford, 2007). In some works of science fiction and fantasy, empathy is understood to be a paranormal or psychic ability to sense the emotions of others, as opposed to telepathy, which allows one to perceive thoughts as well. A person who has that ability is also called an "empath" or "telepath" in this context. Occasionally these empaths are also able to project their own emotions, or to affect the emotions of others.[citation needed]

History

Some postmodern historians such as Keith Jenkins in recent years have debated whether or not it is possible to empathise with people from the past. Jenkins argues that empathy only enjoys such a privileged position in the present because it corresponds harmoniously with the dominant Liberal discourse of modern society and can be connected to John Stuart Mill's concept of reciprocal freedom. Jenkins argues the past is a foreign country and as we do not have access to the epistemological conditions of by gone ages we are unable to empathise.[92]

It is impossible to forecast the effect of empathy on the future.[citation needed] A past subject may take part in the present by the so-called historic present. If we watch from a fictitious past, can tell the present with the future tense, as it happens with the trick of the false prophecy. There is no way of telling the present with the means of the past.[93]

Business

In the 2009 book Wired to Care, strategy consultant Dev Patnaik argues that a major flaw in contemporary business practice is a lack of empathy inside large corporations. He states that lacking any sense of empathy, people inside companies struggle to make intuitive decisions and often get fooled into believing they understand their business if they have quantitative research to rely upon. Patnaik claims that the real opportunity for companies doing business in the 21st Century is to create a widely held sense of empathy for customers, pointing to Nike, Harley-Davidson, and IBM as examples of "Open Empathy Organizations". Such institutions, he claims, see new opportunities more quickly than competitors, adapt to change more easily, and create workplaces that offer employees a greater sense of mission in their jobs [4]. In the 2011 book The Empathy Factor, organizational consultant Marie Miyashiro similarly argues the value of bringing empathy to the workplace, and offers Nonviolent Communication as an effective mechanism for achieving this.[94]

Philosophy

In the 2007 book The Ethics of Care and Empathy, philosopher Michael Slote introduces a theory of care-based ethics that is grounded in empathy. His claim is that moral motivation does, and should, stem from a basis of empathic response. He claims that our natural reaction to situations of moral significance are explained by empathy. He explains that the limits and obligations of empathy and in turn morality are natural. These natural obligations include a greater empathic, and moral obligation to family and friends, along with an account of temporal and physical distance. In situations of close temporal and physical distance, and with family or friends, our moral obligation seems stronger to us than with strangers at a distance naturally. Slote explains that this is due to empathy and our natural empathic ties. He further adds that actions are wrong if and only if they reflect or exhibit a deficiency of fully developed empathic concern for others on the part of the agent.[95]

In phenomenology, empathy describes the experience of something from the other's viewpoint, without confusion between self and other. This draws on the sense of agency. In the most basic sense, this is the experience of the other's body and, in this sense, it is an experience of "my body over there". In most other respects, however, the experience is modified so that what is experienced is experienced as being the other's experience; in experiencing empathy, what is experienced is not "my" experience, even though I experience it. Empathy is also considered to be the condition of intersubjectivity and, as such, the source of the constitution of objectivity.[citation needed]

Measurement

Research into the measurement of empathy has sought to answer a number of questions: who should be carrying out the measurement? What should pass for empathy and what should be discounted? What unit of measure (UOM) should be adopted and to what degree should each occurrence precisely match that UOM are also key questions that researchers have sought to investigate.

Researchers have approached the measurement of empathy from a number of perspectives.

Behavioural measures normally involve raters assessing the presence or absence of certain either predetermined or ad-hoc behaviours in the subjects they are monitoring. Both verbal and non-verbal behaviours have been captured on video by experimenters such as Truax (1967b).[96] Other experimenters, including Mehrabian and Epstein (1972),[97] have required subjects to comment upon their own feelings and behaviours, or those of other people involved in the experiment, as indirect ways of signalling their level of empathic functioning to the raters.

Physiological responses tend to be captured by elaborate electronic equipment that has been physically connected to the subject's body. Researchers then draw inferences about that person's empathic reactions from the electronic readings produced (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992;[98] Leslie et al., 2004[99]).

Bodily or "somatic" measures can be looked upon as behavioural measures at a micro level. Their focus is upon measuring empathy through facial and other non-verbally expressed reactions in the empathiser. These changes are presumably underpinned by physiological changes brought about by some form of "emotional contagion" or mirroring (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992*; Leslie et al., 2004*). It should be pointed out that these reactions, whilst appearing to reflect the internal emotional state of the empathiser, could also, if the stimulus incident lasted more than the briefest period, be reflecting the results of emotional reactions that are based upon more pieces of thinking through (cognitions) associated with role-taking ("if I were him I would feel...").

Paper-based indices involve one or more of a variety of methods of responding. In some experiments, subjects are required to watch video scenarios (either staged or authentic) and to make written responses which are then assessed for their levels of empathy (e.g. Geher, Warner and Brown, 2001[100]); scenarios are sometimes also depicted in printed form (e.g. Mehrabian and Epstein, 1972[101]). Measures also frequently require subjects to self-report upon their own ability or capacity for empathy, using Likert-style numerical responses to a printed questionnaire that may have been designed to tap into the emotional, cognitive-affective or largely cognitive substrates of empathic functioning. Some questionnaires claim to have been able to tap into both cognitive and emotional substrates (e.g. Davis, 1980[102]). More recent paper-based tools include The Empathy Quotient (EQ) created by Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright[103] which comprises a self-report questionnaire consisting of 60 items.

For the very young, picture or puppet-story indices for empathy have been adopted to enable even very young, pre-school subjects to respond without needing to read questions and write answers (e.g. Denham and Couchoud, 1990). Dependent variables (variables that are monitored for any change by the experimenter) for younger subjects have included self reporting on a 7-point smiley face scale and filmed facial reactions (Barnett, 1984).[104]

A certain amount of confusion exists about how to measure empathy. These may be rooted in another problem: deciding what is empathy and what is not. In general, researchers have until now been keen to pin down a singular definition of empathy which would allow them to design a measure to assess its presence in an exchange, in someone's repertoire of behaviours or within them as a latent trait. As a result they have been frequently forced to ignore the richness of the empathic process in favour of capturing surface, explicit self-report or third-party data about whether empathy between two people was present or not. In most cases, instruments have unfortunately only yielded information on whether someone had the potential to demonstrate empathy (Geher et al., 2001)*. Gladstein (1987)[105] summarises the position noting that empathy has been measured from the point of view of the empathiser, the recipient for empathy and the third-party observer. He suggests that since the multiple measures used have produced results that bear little relation to one another, researchers should refrain from making comparisons between scales that are in fact measuring different things. He suggests that researchers should instead stipulate what kind of empathy they are setting out to measure rather than simplistically stating that they are setting out to measure the unitary phenomenon "empathy"; a view more recently endorsed by Duan and Hill (1996).[106]

In the field of medicine, a measurement tool for carers is the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, Health Professional Version (JSPE-HP).[107] At least one study using this tool with health sciences' students has found that levels of empathy are greater amongst females than males, and also are greater amongst older students than younger students.[108]

Gender differences

The issue of gender differences in empathy is quite controversial. It is often believed that females are more empathic than males. Evidence for gender differences in empathy are important for self-report questionnaires of empathy in which it is obvious what was being indexed (e.g., impact of social desirability and gender stereotypes) but are smaller or nonexistent for other types of indexes that are less self-evident with regard to their purpose.[109] On average female subjects score higher than males on the Empathy Quotient (EQ), while males tend to score higher on the Systemizing Quotient (SQ).

Both males and females with Autistic Spectrum Disorders usually score higher on the SQ (Baron-Cohen, 2003).[24] However, a series of recent studies, using a variety of neurophysiological measures, including MEG,[110] spinal reflex excitability,[111] and electroencephalography[112][113] have documented the presence of a gender difference in the human mirror neuron system, with female participants exhibiting stronger motor resonance than male participants. In addition, these aforementioned studies found that female participants scored higher on empathy self-report dispositional measures and that these measures positively correlated with the physiological response. However, other studies show that women do not possess greater empathic abilities than men, and perceived gender differences are the result of motivational differences.[114][115] Using fMRI, neuroscientist Tania Singer showed that empathy-related neural responses are significantly lower in males when observing an "unfair" person experiencing pain.[116]

See also

2

References

  1. ^ "Empathy". plato.stanford.edu. 2008-03-31. Retrieved 2012-08-29.
  2. ^ Empatheia, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, at Perseus
  3. ^ Gallese, Vittorio (2003). "The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity". Psychopathology. 36: 171–180. doi:10.1159/000072786. PMID 14504450. CiteSeerx10.1.1.143.2396. {{cite journal}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help)
  4. ^ Bar-On, Reuven; Parker, James DA (2000). The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace. San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 0787949841. pp. 40-59
  5. ^ Taylor (1997), pp. 28–31
  6. ^ Hodges, S.D., & Klein, K.J. (2001). Regulating the costs of empathy: the price of being human. Journal of Socio-Economics.
  7. ^ Batson C. D., Fultz J., Schoenrade P. (1987). "Distress and Empathy: Two Qualitatively Distinct Vicarious Emotions with Different Motivational Consequences". Journal of Personality. 55: 19–39.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  8. ^ Berger, D. M. (1987). Clinical empathy. Northvale: Jason Aronson, Inc.
  9. ^ Decety J., Jackson P.L. (2004). "The functional architecture of human empathy". Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews. 3: 71–100. doi:10.1177/1534582304267187. PMID 15537986.
  10. ^ Decety, J., & Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 1053–1080.
  11. ^ a b c de Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 279-300.
  12. ^ Eisenberg N., Fabes R.A. (1990). "Empathy: conceptualization, measurement, and relation to prosocial behavior". Motivation and Emotion. 14: 131–149.
  13. ^ Davis,Carol M. (1990). "What Is Empathy, and Can Empathy Be Taught?". Physical Therapy. 70.11: 707–715.
  14. ^ Goldman A (1993). "Ethics and cognitive science". Ethics. 103: 337–360.
  15. ^ Hoffman, Martin (1984). "Interaction of affect and cognition in empathy". In Izard, C.; Kagan, J..; Zajonc, R.. (eds.). Emotions, Cognition, and Behavior. Cambridge University Press. pp. 103–131.
  16. ^ Ickes, William (1997). Empathetic Accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press.
  17. ^ Kohut, Heinz; Goldberg, Arnold, and Stepansky, Paul E.. How Does Analysis Cure?. University of Chicago Press..,
  18. ^ "Interview with Harry Prosen, M.D., Psychiatric Consultant Bonobo Species Survival Plan". Retrieved 11 August 2011.
  19. ^ Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality and interpersonal relationships, as developed in the client-centered framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of science (Vol. 3, pp. 210-211; 184-256). New York: McGraw Hill.
  20. ^ Rosenberg, Marshall B. (2005). "5: Connecting With Others Empathically". Speak Peace in a World of Conflict: What You Say Next Will Change Your World. Puddledancer Press. p. 240. ISBN 978-1-892005-17-5.
  21. ^ Schafer R (1959). "Generative empathy in the treatment situation". The Psychoanalytic Quarterly. 28: 342–373.
  22. ^ Schwartz W (2002). "From passivity to competence: A conceptualization of knowledge, skill, tolerance, and empathy". Psychiatry. 65 (4): 338–345.
  23. ^ Zum Problem der Einfühlung (On the Problem of Empathy) (1916), translated by Waltraut Stein 1989
  24. ^ a b c d The Essential Difference: The Truth about the Male and Female Brain, Basic Books (July 1, 2003) ISBN 978-0-7382-0844-2
  25. ^ Traditions of Compassion: From Religious Duty to Social Activism (2005)
  26. ^ Happiness Genes: Unlock the Positive Potential Hidden in Your DNA, New Page Books (April, 2010) ISBN 978-1-60163-105-3
  27. ^ O'Malley, W. J. (1999). Teaching Empathy. America, 180(12), 22-26.
  28. ^ Meltzoff, A.N., & Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. The Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London, 358, 491-500.
  29. ^ Batson, C.D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In J. Decety and W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (pp. 3-15). Cambridge: MIT Press
  30. ^ Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. L. & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 2, 96-99. http://www.elainehatfield.com/ch50.pdf
  31. ^ a b c Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (2007). "Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome". J Autism Dev Disord. 37 (4): 709–15. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  32. ^ a b Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Judith Aharon-Peretz and Daniella Perry (2009). "Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions". Brain. 132 (3): 617–627. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279.
  33. ^ Frans B.M. deWaal (2008). "Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy" (PDF). Annu. Rev. Psychol. (59): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625.
  34. ^ Kimberley Rogers, Isabel Dziobek, Jason Hassenstab, Oliver T. Wolf, Antonio Convit (2007). "Who Cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord (37): 709–715. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  35. ^ a b c Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. (2012). "The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics". Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 7 (6): 727–37. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr051. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |month= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  36. ^ Yan Fan, Niall W. Duncana, Moritz de Greck, Georg Northoff (2011). "Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 35 (3): 903–911. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. ^ a b c d e f Ilaria Minio-Paluello, Michael V. Lombardo, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Sally Wheelwright, Simon Baron-Cohen (2009). "Response to Smith's Letter to the Editor 'Emotional Empathy in Autism Spectrum Conditions: Weak, Intact, or Heightened?'" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0800-x.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  38. ^ a b Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural basis of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19: 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  39. ^ Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  40. ^ Decety J., Meyer M. (2008). "From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account". Development and Psychopathology. 20: 1053–1080.
  41. ^ Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006) Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (pp. 517-549). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  42. ^ Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people's action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9
  43. ^ Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1990). The origins of empathic concern. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 107-130.
  44. ^ Decety J., Michalska K.J., Akitsuki Y. (2008). "Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children". Neuropsychologia. 46: 2607–2614.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  45. ^ Brain Scans Show Children Naturally Prone to Empathy Newswise, retrieved on July 13, 2008.
  46. ^ Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103-28.
  47. ^ Olsen, Douglas (September 2001). "Empathetic Maturity: Theory of Moral Point of View in Clinical Relations". Advances in Nursing Science. 24 (1): 36–46. PMID 11554532.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: date and year (link)
  48. ^ Keysers, C. & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions and sensations. Curr Opinion Neurobiol, 2009
  49. ^ Decety, J., & Moriguchi, Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. BioPsychoSocial Medicine, 1, 22-65.
  50. ^ Wicker B.; et al. (2003). "Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40: 655–664. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  51. ^ Keysers, C. et al. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch, Neuron, 42:335-46
  52. ^ Blakemore S.-J.; et al. (2005). "Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia". Brain. 128: 1571–1583. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  53. ^ Morrison, I., Lloyd, D., di Pellegrino, G., & Roberts, N. (2004). Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue? Cognitive & Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 270-278.
  54. ^ Jackson P.L., Meltzoff A.N., Decety J. (2005). "How do we perceive the pain of others: A window into the neural processes involved in empathy". NeuroImage. 24: 771–779.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  55. ^ Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19: 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  56. ^ Singer T.; et al. (2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain". Science. 303: 1157–1161. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  57. ^ Preston, S., & de Waal, F. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1-72.
  58. ^ Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during obser- vation of outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (2010), doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011
  59. ^ http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811912010646
  60. ^ http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2012/10/121030161416.htm
  61. ^ a b "Abstract: Empathy and justice motivation, Martin L. Hoffman". Springerlink.com. Retrieved 2012-08-29.
  62. ^ Abstract: Assessing a new dimension of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and punishing desires, Vitaglione, Guy D. & Barnett, Mark A. [1]
  63. ^ Mohr P., Howells K., Gerace A., Day A., Wharton M. (2007). "The role of perspective taking in anger arousal". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (3): 507–517.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  64. ^ a b c Decety, J., Michalska, K.J., Akitsuki, Y., & Lahey, B. (2008). "Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation". Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–11. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC 2819310. PMID 18940230. {{cite journal}}: More than one of |work= and |journal= specified (help); Unknown parameter |note= ignored (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  65. ^ Hill, E., Berthoz, S., & Frith, U (2004) 'Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives.' Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, (2) 229-235.
  66. ^ Taylor, G.J. and Bagby, R.M & Parker, J.D.A. Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. (1997) Cambridge Uni. Press.
  67. ^ a b Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. Empathy and judging other's pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex (2007); Bird, J., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U., and Frith, C. Alexithymia In Autistic Spectrum Disorders: and fMRI Investigation (2006)  : and Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U & C. Alexithymia in Autism Spectrum Disorders: an fMRI Investigation (2006).
  68. ^ Brackett et al. 'Emotional Intelligence and Relationship Quality Among Couples' in Personal Relationships, 12 (2005) p.197-212 [2]
  69. ^ Yelsma, P., Marrow, S. - 'An Examination of Couples' Difficulties With Emotional Expressiveness and Their Marital Satisfaction' in Journal of Family Communication 3 (2003) p.41-62 [3]
  70. ^ Allen. - 'Autism, music, and the therapeutic potential of music in alexithymia' in Music Perception (2010) p.251
  71. ^ IMFAR 2007 abstract http://www.autism-insar.org/docs/IMFAR2007_Program.pdf
  72. ^ Dapretto M., Davies M.S., Pfeifer J.H., Scott A.A., Sigman M., Bookheimer S.Y., Iacoboni M. (2006). "Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 9: 28–31. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMID 16327784.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  73. ^ Oberman. - 'EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum' in Brain Research (2005) p.190
  74. ^ Phoebe Caldwell, "Letters", London Times, Dec 30 2005
  75. ^ Baron-Cohen. - 'Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism' in Science (2005) p.819
  76. ^ "Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism".
  77. ^ Tunstall N., Fahy T. and McGuire P. in Guide to Neuroimaging in Psychiatry, Eds. Fu C et al., Martin Dunitz: London 2003.
  78. ^ Narcissistic personality disorderDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
  79. ^ Schizoid personality disorder, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
  80. ^ http://www.mentalhealth.com/icd/p22-pe02.html Schizoid personality disorder - International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10)]
  81. ^ Guntrip, Harry. Schizoid Phenomena, Object-Relations, and The Self. New York: International Universities Press, 1969.
  82. ^ Ralph Klein- pp. 13–23 in Disorders of the Self: New Therapeutic Horizons, Brunner/Mazel (1995).
  83. ^ Wyschogrod E (1981). "Empathy and sympathy as tactile encounter". J Med Philos. 6 (1): 25–43.
  84. ^ http://www.wired.co.uk/news/archive/2012-07/12/tania-singer-compassion-burnout
  85. ^ Aronson, Elliot (2007). Social Psychology, 6th Edition. Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-238245-8. {{cite book}}: Unknown parameter |coauthors= ignored (|author= suggested) (help)
  86. ^ Sandin, Jo (2007). Bonobos: Encounters in Empathy. Milwaukee: Zoological Society of Milwaukee & The Foundation for Wildlife Conservation, Inc. p. 109. ISBN 978-0-9794151-0-4.
  87. ^ The age of empathy: nature's lessons for a kinder society By: Waal, F. B. M. de. Harmony Books 2009
  88. ^ Ben-Ami Bartal I., Decety J., Mason P. (2011). "Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats". Science. 334: 1427–1430.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  89. ^ Dale J. Langford, Sara E. Crager, Zarrar Shehzad, Shad B. Smith, Susana G. Sotocinal, Jeremy S. Levenstadt, Mona Lisa Chanda, Daniel J. Levitin, Jeffrey S. Mogil (June 30, 2006). "Social Modulation of Pain as Evidence for Empathy in Mice". Science. 312 (5782): 1967–1970. doi:10.1126/science.1128322. PMID 16809545.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  90. ^ Decety J (2011). "The neuroevolution of empathy". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1231: 35–45. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x. PMID 21651564.
  91. ^ Decety, J., & Svetlova, M. (2012). Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 1-24
  92. ^ Jenkins, K. (1991) Re-thinking History London: Routledge
  93. ^ Pozzi, G. (1976) Prefazione 6. L'elemento storico e politico -sociale, in G.B. Marino, L'Adone Milano
  94. ^ Miyashiro, Marie R. (2011). The Empathy Factor: Your Competitive Advantage for Personal, Team, and Business Success. Puddledancer Press. p. 256. ISBN 1-892005-25-5.
  95. ^ The Ethics of Care and Empathy, Michael Slote, Oxford University Press, 2007
  96. ^ Truax, C. B. (1967). Rating of Accurate Empathy. The Therapeutic Relationship and its Impact. A Study of Psychotherapy with Schizophrenics. Eds. C. R. Rogers, E. T. Gendlin, D. J. Kiesler and C. B. Truax. Madison, Wisconsin, The University of Wisconsin Press pp. 555-568.
  97. ^ Mehrabian, A. and N. Epstein (1972). "A measure of emotional empathy." Journal of Personality 40: 525-543.
  98. ^ Levenson R. W., Ruef A. M. (1992). "Empathy: a physiological substrate". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63: 234–246.
  99. ^ Leslie K. R., Johnson-Frey S. H.; et al. (2004). "Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy". NeuroImage. 21: 601–607. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  100. ^ Geher G., Warner R. M.; et al. (2001). "Predictive validity of the emotional accuracy research scale". Intelligence. 29: 373–388. {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  101. ^ Mehrabian A., Epstein N. (1972). "A measure of emotional empathy". Journal of Personality. 40: 525–543.
  102. ^ Davis M. H. (1980). "A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy". JSAS Catalogue of selected documents in psychology. 10 (4): 1–17.
  103. ^ Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. (2004). "The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34: 163–175.
  104. ^ Barnett M. A. (1984). "Similarity of experience and empathy in preschoolers". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 145: 241–250.
  105. ^ Gladstein, G. A. (1987). What It All Means. Empathy and counseling: explorations in theory and research. G. A. Gladstein. New York, Springer-Verlag: 173-189.
  106. ^ Duan C., Hill C. E. (1996). "The current state of empathy research". Journal of Counselling Psychology. 43: 261–274.
  107. ^ Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J (2007). "A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy". J Gen Intern Med. 22 (10): 1434–8. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0298-x. PMC 2305857. PMID 17653807. {{cite journal}}: |access-date= requires |url= (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  108. ^ Boyle M; et al. (2010). "Levels of empathy in undergraduate health science students". The Internet Journal of Medical Education. 1 (1). {{cite journal}}: Explicit use of et al. in: |author= (help)
  109. ^ Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R.A. (1990). Empathy: Conceptualization, Measurement, and Relation to Prosocial Behavior. Motivation and Emotion, 14, 131–149.
  110. ^ Cheng Y., Tzeng O.J., Decety J., Hsieh J.C. (2006). "Gender differences in the human mirror system: a magnetoencephalography study". NeuroReport. 17: 1115–1119. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21. PMID 16837838.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  111. ^ Cheng Y., Decety J., Hsieh J.C., Hung D., Tzeng O.J. (2007). "Gender differences in spinal excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion". NeuroReport. 18: 887–890. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280ebb486. PMID 17515795.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  112. ^ Yang C.Y., Decety J., Lee S., Chen G., Cheng Y. (2009). "Gender differences in the Mu rhythm during empathy for pain: An electroencephalographic study". Brain Research. 1251: 176–184.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  113. ^ Cheng Y., Lee P., Yang C.Y., Lin C.P., Decety J. (2008). "Gender differences in the mu rhythm of the human mirror-neuron system". PLoS ONE. 5: e2113.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  114. ^ Ickes, W. (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press.
  115. ^ Klein K. Hodges S. (2001). "Gender Differences, Motivation, and Empathic Accuracy: When it Pays to Understand". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27 (6): 720–730. doi:10.1177/0146167201276007.
  116. ^ Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ, Frith CD.Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature. 2006 Jan 26; 439:466-9.

External links