Jump to content

Camouflage

This is a good article. Click here for more information.
From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by Citation bot (talk | contribs) at 00:47, 8 October 2018 (Alter: isbn, pages, journal, template type, title. Add: title-link, volume, journal, date, hdl, pages, pmc, pmid. Removed parameters. Formatted dashes. You can use this bot yourself. Report bugs here. | Josve05a). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

The peacock flounder can change its pattern and colours to match its environment.
photo of a soldier putting on camouflage face paint
A soldier applying camouflage face paint; both helmet and jacket are disruptively patterned.

Camouflage is the use of any combination of materials, coloration, or illumination for concealment, either by making animals or objects hard to see (crypsis), or by disguising them as something else (mimesis). Examples include the leopard's spotted coat, the battledress of a modern soldier, and the leaf-mimic katydid's wings. A third approach, motion dazzle, confuses the observer with a conspicuous pattern, making the object visible but momentarily harder to locate. The majority of camouflage methods aim for crypsis, often through a general resemblance to the background, high contrast disruptive coloration, eliminating shadow, and countershading. In the open ocean, where there is no background, the principal methods of camouflage are transparency, silvering, and countershading, while the ability to produce light is among other things used for counter-illumination on the undersides of cephalopods such as squid. Some animals, such as chameleons and octopuses, are capable of actively changing their skin pattern and colours, whether for camouflage or for signalling. It is possible that some plants use camouflage to evade being eaten by herbivores.

Military camouflage was spurred by the increasing range and accuracy of firearms in the 19th century. In particular the replacement of the inaccurate musket with the rifle made personal concealment in battle a survival skill. In the 20th century, military camouflage developed rapidly, especially during the First World War. On land, artists such as André Mare designed camouflage schemes and observation posts disguised as trees. At sea, merchant ships and troop carriers were painted in dazzle patterns that were highly visible, but designed to confuse enemy submarines as to the target's speed, range, and heading. During and after the Second World War, a variety of camouflage schemes were used for aircraft and for ground vehicles in different theatres of war. The use of radar since the mid-20th century has largely made camouflage for fixed-wing military aircraft obsolete.

Non-military use of camouflage includes making cell telephone towers less obtrusive and helping hunters to approach wary game animals. Patterns derived from military camouflage are frequently used in fashion clothing, exploiting their strong designs and sometimes their symbolism. Camouflage themes recur in modern art, and both figuratively and literally in science fiction and works of literature.

History

Octopuses like this Octopus cyanea can change colour (and shape) for camouflage

In ancient Greece, Aristotle (384–322 BC) commented on the colour-changing abilities, both for camouflage and for signalling, of cephalopods including the octopus, in his Historia animalium:[1]

The octopus ... seeks its prey by so changing its colour as to render it like the colour of the stones adjacent to it; it does so also when alarmed.

— Aristotle.[1]

Camouflage has been a topic of interest and research in zoology for well over a century. According to Charles Darwin's 1859 theory of natural selection,[2] features such as camouflage evolved by providing individual animals with a reproductive advantage, enabling them to leave more offspring, on average, than other members of the same species. In his Origin of Species, Darwin wrote:[3]

When we see leaf-eating insects green, and bark-feeders mottled-grey; the alpine ptarmigan white in winter, the red-grouse the colour of heather, and the black-grouse that of peaty earth, we must believe that these tints are of service to these birds and insects in preserving them from danger. Grouse, if not destroyed at some period of their lives, would increase in countless numbers; they are known to suffer largely from birds of prey; and hawks are guided by eyesight to their prey, so much so, that on parts of the Continent persons are warned not to keep white pigeons, as being the most liable to destruction. Hence I can see no reason to doubt that natural selection might be most effective in giving the proper colour to each kind of grouse, and in keeping that colour, when once acquired, true and constant.[3]

Experiment by Poulton, 1890: swallowtailed moth pupae with camouflage they acquired as larvae

The English zoologist Edward Bagnall Poulton studied animal coloration, especially camouflage. In his 1890 book The Colours of Animals, he classified different types such as "special protective resemblance" (where an animal looks like another object), or "general aggressive resemblance" (where a predator blends in with the background, enabling it to approach prey). His experiments showed that swallowtailed moth pupae were camouflaged to match the backgrounds on which they were reared as larvae.[4][a] Poulton's "general protective resemblance"[6] was at that time considered to be the main method of camouflage, as when Frank Evers Beddard wrote in 1892 that "tree-frequenting animals are often green in colour. Among vertebrates numerous species of parrots, iguanas, tree-frogs, and the green tree-snake are examples".[7] Beddard did however briefly mention other methods, including the "alluring coloration" of the flower mantis and the possibility of a different mechanism in the orange tip butterfly. He wrote that "the scattered green spots upon the under surface of the wings might have been intended for a rough sketch of the small flowerets of the plant [an umbellifer], so close is their mutual resemblance."[8][b] He also explained the coloration of sea fish such as the mackerel: "Among pelagic fish it is common to find the upper surface dark-coloured and the lower surface white, so that the animal is inconspicuous when seen either from above or below."[10]

Abbott Thayer's 1907 painting Peacock in the Woods depicted a peacock as if it were camouflaged.

The artist Abbott Handerson Thayer formulated what is sometimes called Thayer's Law, the principle of countershading.[11] However, he overstated the case in the 1909 book Concealing-Coloration in the Animal Kingdom, arguing that "All patterns and colors whatsoever of all animals that ever preyed or are preyed on are under certain normal circumstances obliterative" (that is, cryptic camouflage), and that "Not one 'mimicry' mark, not one 'warning color'... nor any 'sexually selected' color, exists anywhere in the world where there is not every reason to believe it the very best conceivable device for the concealment of its wearer",[12][13] and using paintings such as Peacock in the Woods (1907) to reinforce his argument.[14] Thayer was roundly mocked for these views by critics including Teddy Roosevelt.[15]

The English zoologist Hugh Cott's 1940 book Adaptive Coloration in Animals corrected Thayer's errors, sometimes sharply: "Thus we find Thayer straining the theory to a fantastic extreme in an endeavour to make it cover almost every type of coloration in the animal kingdom."[16] Cott built on Thayer's discoveries, developing a comprehensive view of camouflage based on "maximum disruptive contrast", countershading and hundreds of examples. The book explained how disruptive camouflage worked, using streaks of boldly contrasting colour, paradoxically making objects less visible by breaking up their outlines.[17] While Cott was more systematic and balanced in his view than Thayer, and did include some experimental evidence on the effectiveness of camouflage,[18] his 500-page textbook was, like Thayer's, mainly a natural history narrative which illustrated theories with examples.[19]

Camouflage is a soft-tissue feature that is rarely preserved in the fossil record, but rare fossilised skin samples from the Cretaceous period show that some marine reptiles were countershaded. The skins, pigmented with dark-coloured eumelanin, reveal that both leatherback turtles and mosasaurs had dark backs and light bellies.[20]

Experimental evidence that camouflage helps prey avoid being detected by predators was first provided in 2016, when ground-nesting birds (plovers and coursers) were shown to survive according to how well their egg contrast matched the local environment.[21]

Principles

photo of a Draco dussumieri on a tree trunk, very hard to see
Draco dussumieri uses several methods of camouflage, including disruptive coloration, lying flat, and concealment of shadow.

Camouflage can be achieved by different methods, described below. Most of the methods contribute to crypsis, helping to hide against a background; but mimesis and motion dazzle protect without hiding. Methods may be applied on their own or in combination.

Crypsis

Crypsis means making the animal or military equipment hard to see (or to detect in other ways, such as by sound or scent). Visual crypsis can be achieved in many different ways, such as by living underground or by being active only at night, as well as by a variety of methods of camouflage.[22]

Resemblance to surroundings

Some animals' colours and patterns resemble a particular natural background. This is an important component of camouflage in all environments. For instance, tree-dwelling parakeets are mainly green; woodcocks of the forest floor are brown and speckled; reedbed bitterns are streaked brown and buff; in each case the animal's coloration matches the hues of its habitat.[23][24] Similarly, desert animals are almost all desert coloured in tones of sand, buff, ochre, and brownish grey, whether they are mammals like the gerbil or fennec fox, birds such as the desert lark or sandgrouse, or reptiles like the skink or horned viper.[25]Military uniforms, too, generally resemble their backgrounds; for example khaki uniforms are a muddy or dusty colour, originally chosen for service in South Asia.[26]Many[27] moths show industrial melanism, including the peppered moth which has coloration that blends in with tree bark.[28] The coloration of these insects evolved between 1860 and 1940 to match the changing colour of the tree trunks on which they rest, from pale and mottled to almost black in polluted areas.[27][c] This is taken by zoologists as evidence that camouflage is influenced by natural selection, as well as demonstrating that it changes where necessary to resemble the local background.[27]

Disruptive coloration

Illustration of the principle of "maximum disruptive contrast" by Hugh Cott, 1940

Disruptive patterns use strongly contrasting, non-repeating markings such as spots or stripes to break up the outlines of an animal or military vehicle,[29] or to conceal telltale features, especially by masking the eyes, as in the common frog.[30] Disruptive patterns may use more than one method to defeat visual systems such as edge detection.[31] Predators like the leopard use disruptive camouflage to help them approach prey, while potential prey like the Egyptian nightjar use it to avoid detection by predators.[32] Disruptive patterning is common in military usage, both for uniforms and for military vehicles. Disruptive patterning, however, does not always achieve crypsis on its own, as an animal or a military target may be given away by factors like shape, shine, and shadow.[33][34][35]

The presence of bold skin markings does not in itself prove that an animal relies on camouflage, as that depends on its behaviour.[36] For example, although giraffes have a high contrast pattern that could be disruptive coloration, the adults are very conspicuous when in the open. Some authors have argued that adult giraffes are cryptic, since when standing among trees and bushes they are hard to see at even a few metres distance.[37] However, adult giraffes move about to gain the best view of an approaching predator, relying on their size and ability to defend themselves, even from lions, rather than on camouflage.[37] A different explanation is implied by young giraffes being far more vulnerable to predation than adults: more than half of all giraffe calves die within a year,[37] and giraffe mothers hide their calves, which spend much of the time lying down in cover while their mothers are away feeding. Since the presence of a mother nearby does not affect survival, it is argued that young giraffes must be very well camouflaged; this is supported by coat markings being strongly inherited.[37]

The possibility of camouflage in plants has been little studied until the late 20th century. Leaf variegation with white spots may serve as camouflage in forest understory plants, where there is a dappled background; leaf mottling is correlated with closed habitats. Disruptive camouflage would have a clear evolutionary advantage in plants: they would tend to escape from being eaten by herbivores. Another possibility is that some plants have leaves differently coloured on upper and lower surfaces or on parts such as veins and stalks to make green-camouflaged insects conspicuous, and thus benefit the plants by favouring the removal of herbivores by carnivores. These hypotheses are testable.[38][39][40]

Eliminating shadow

Camouflaged animals and vehicles are readily given away by their shapes and shadows. A flange helps to hide the shadow and a pale fringe breaks up and averages out any shadow that remains.

Some animals, such as the horned lizards of North America, have evolved elaborate measures to eliminate shadow. Their bodies are flattened, with the sides thinning to an edge; the animals habitually press their bodies to the ground; and their sides are fringed with white scales which effectively hide and disrupt any remaining areas of shadow there may be under the edge of the body.[41] The theory that the body shape of the horned lizards which live in open desert is adapted to minimise shadow is supported by the one species which lacks fringe scales, the roundtail horned lizard, which lives in rocky areas and resembles a rock. When this species is threatened, it makes itself look as much like a rock as possible by curving its back, emphasizing its three-dimensional shape.[41] Some species of butterflies, such as the speckled wood, Pararge aegeria, minimise their shadows when perched by closing the wings over their backs, aligning their bodies with the sun, and tilting to one side towards the sun, so that the shadow becomes a thin inconspicuous line rather than a broad patch.[42] Similarly, some ground-nesting birds including the European nightjar select a resting position facing the sun.[42] Eliminating shadow was identified as a principle of military camouflage during the Second World War.[43]

Distraction

Conspicuous white distractive markings of bushbuck (Tragelaphus scriptus) take a predator's attention from recognising the prey.

Many prey animals have conspicuous high-contrast markings which paradoxically attract the predator's gaze.[d][44] These distractive markings serve as camouflage by distracting the predator's attention from recognising the prey as a whole, for example by keeping the predator from identifying the prey's outline. Experimentally, search times for blue tits increased when artificial prey had distractive markings.[45]

Self-decoration

Some animals actively seek to hide by decorating themselves with materials such as twigs, sand, or pieces of shell from their environment, to break up their outlines, to conceal the features of their bodies, and to match their backgrounds. For example, a caddis fly larva builds a decorated case and lives almost entirely inside it; a decorator crab covers its back with seaweed, sponges and stones.[22] The nymph of the predatory masked bug uses its hind legs and a 'tarsal fan' to decorate its body with sand or dust. There are two layers of bristles (trichomes) over the body. On these, the nymph spreads an inner layer of fine particles and an outer layer of coarser particles. The camouflage may conceal the bug from both predators and prey.[46][47]

Similar principles can be applied for military purposes, for instance when a sniper wears a ghillie suit designed to be further camouflaged by decoration with materials such as tufts of grass from the sniper's immediate environment. Such suits were used as early as 1916, the British army having adopted "coats of motley hue and stripes of paint" for snipers.[48] Cott takes the example of the larva of the blotched emerald moth, which fixes a screen of fragments of leaves to its specially hooked bristles, to argue that military camouflage uses the same method, pointing out that the "device is ... essentially the same as one widely practised during the Great War for the concealment, not of caterpillars, but of caterpillar-tractors, [gun] battery positions, observation posts and so forth."[49][50]

Cryptic behaviour

The leafy sea dragon sways like seaweeds to reinforce its camouflage.

Movement catches the eye of prey animals on the lookout for predators, and of predators hunting for prey.[51] Most methods of crypsis therefore also require suitable cryptic behaviour, such as lying down and keeping still to avoid being detected, or in the case of stalking predators such as the tiger, moving with extreme stealth, both slowly and quietly, watching its prey for any sign they are aware of its presence.[51] As an example of the combination of behaviours and other methods of crypsis involved, young giraffes seek cover, lie down, and keep still, often for hours until their mothers return; their skin pattern blends with the pattern of the vegetation, while the chosen cover and lying position together hide the animals' shadows.[37] The flat-tail horned lizard similarly relies on a combination of methods: it is adapted to lie flat in the open desert, relying on stillness, its cryptic coloration, and concealment of its shadow to avoid being noticed by predators.[52] In the ocean, the leafy sea dragon sways mimetically, like the seaweeds amongst which it rests, as if rippled by wind or water currents.[53] Swaying is seen also in some insects, like Macleay's Spectre stick insect, Extatosoma tiaratum. The behaviour may be motion crypsis, preventing detection, or motion masquerade, promoting misclassification (as something other than prey), or a combination of the two.[54]

Motion camouflage

Comparison of motion camouflage and classical pursuit

Most forms of camouflage are ineffective when the camouflaged animal or object moves, because the motion is easily seen by the observing predator, prey or enemy.[55] However, insects such as hoverflies[56] and dragonflies use motion camouflage: the hoverflies to approach possible mates, and the dragonflies to approach rivals when defending territories.[57][58] Motion camouflage is achieved by moving so as to stay on a straight line between the target and a fixed point in the landscape; the pursuer thus appears not to move, but only to loom larger in the target's field of vision.[59] The same method can be used for military purposes, for example by missiles to minimise their risk of detection by an enemy.[56] However, missile engineers, and animals such as bats, use the method mainly for its efficiency rather than camouflage.[60]

Fish and frog melanophore cells change colour by moving pigment-containing bodies.

Changeable skin coloration

Animals such as chameleon, frog,[61] flatfish such as the peacock flounder, squid and octopus actively change their skin patterns and colours using special chromatophore cells to resemble their current background, or, as in most chameleons, for signalling.[62] However, Smith's dwarf chameleon does use active colour change for camouflage.[63]

Four frames of the same peacock flounder taken a few minutes apart, showing its ability to match its coloration to the environment

Each chromatophore contains pigment of only one colour. In fish and frogs, colour change is mediated by the type of chromatophores known as melanophores that contain dark pigment. A melanophore is star-shaped; it contains many small pigmented organelles which can be dispersed throughout the cell, or aggregated near its centre. When the pigmented organelles are dispersed, the cell makes a patch of the animal's skin appear dark; when they are aggregated, most of the cell, and the animal's skin, appears light. In frogs, the change is controlled relatively slowly, mainly by hormones. In fish, the change is controlled by the brain, which sends signals directly to the chromatophores, as well as producing hormones.[64]

The skins of cephalopods such as the octopus contain complex units, each consisting of a chromatophore with surrounding muscle and nerve cells.[65] The cephalopod chromatophore has all its pigment grains in a small elastic sac, which can be stretched or allowed to relax under the control of the brain to vary its opacity. By controlling chromatophores of different colours, cephalopods can rapidly change their skin patterns and colours.[66][67]

On a longer timescale, animals like the Arctic hare, Arctic fox, stoat, and rock ptarmigan have snow camouflage, changing their coat colour (by moulting and growing new fur or feathers) from brown or grey in the summer to white in the winter; the Arctic fox is the only species in the dog family to do so.[68] However, Arctic hares which live in the far north of Canada, where summer is very short, remain white year-round.[68][69]

The principle of varying coloration either rapidly or with the changing seasons has military applications. Active camouflage could in theory make use of both dynamic colour change and counterillumination. Simple methods such as changing uniforms and repainting vehicles for winter have been in use since World War II. In 2011, BAE Systems announced their Adaptiv infrared camouflage technology. It uses about 1000 hexagonal panels to cover the sides of a tank. The Peltier plate panels are heated and cooled to match either the vehicle's surroundings (crypsis), or an object such as a car (mimesis), when viewed in infrared.[70][71][72]

Countershading

Countershading acts as a form of camouflage by 'painting out' the self-shadowing of the body or object. The result is a 'flat' appearance, instead of the 'solid' appearance of the body before countershading.

Countershading uses graded colour to counteract the effect of self-shadowing, creating an illusion of flatness. Self-shadowing makes an animal appear darker below than on top, grading from light to dark; countershading 'paints in' tones which are darkest on top, lightest below, making the countershaded animal nearly invisible against a suitable background.[73] Thayer observed that "Animals are painted by Nature, darkest on those parts which tend to be most lighted by the sky's light, and vice versa". Accordingly, the principle of countershading is sometimes called Thayer's Law.[74] Countershading is widely used by terrestrial animals, such as gazelles[75] and grasshoppers; marine animals, such as sharks and dolphins;[76] and birds, such as snipe and dunlin.[77][78]

Countershading is less often used for military camouflage, despite Second World War experiments that showed its effectiveness. English zoologist Hugh Cott encouraged the use of methods including countershading, but despite his authority on the subject, failed to persuade the British authorities.[79] Soldiers often wrongly viewed camouflage netting as a kind of invisibility cloak, and they had to be taught to look at camouflage practically, from an enemy observer's viewpoint.[80][81] At the same time in Australia, zoologist William John Dakin advised soldiers to copy animals' methods, using their instincts for wartime camouflage.[82]

The term countershading has a second meaning unrelated to "Thayer's Law". It is that the upper and undersides of animals such as sharks, and of some military aircraft, are different colours to match the different backgrounds when seen from above or from below. Here the camouflage consists of two surfaces, each with the simple function of providing concealment against a specific background, such as a bright water surface or the sky. The body of a shark or the fuselage of an aircraft is not gradated from light to dark to appear flat when seen from the side. The camouflage methods used are the matching of background colour and pattern, and disruption of outlines.[75]

Counter-illumination

Principle of counter-illumination in the firefly squid

Counter-illumination means producing light to match a background that is brighter than an animal's body or military vehicle; it is a form of active camouflage. It is notably used by some species of squid, such as the firefly squid and the midwater squid. The latter has light-producing organs (photophores) scattered all over its underside; these create a sparkling glow that prevents the animal from appearing as a dark shape when seen from below.[83] Counterillumination camouflage is the likely function of the bioluminescence of many marine organisms, though light is also produced to attract[84] or to detect prey[85] and for signalling.

Counterillumination has rarely been used for military purposes. "Diffused lighting camouflage" was trialled by Canada's National Research Council during the Second World War. It involved projecting light on to the sides of ships to match the faint glow of the night sky, requiring awkward external platforms to support the lamps.[86] The Canadian concept was refined in the American Yehudi lights project, and trialled in aircraft including B-24 Liberators and naval Avengers.[87] The planes were fitted with forward-pointing lamps automatically adjusted to match the brightness of the night sky.[86] This enabled them to approach much closer to a target – within 3,000 yards (2,700 metres) – before being seen.[87] Counterillumination was made obsolete by radar, and neither diffused lighting camouflage nor Yehudi lights entered active service.[86]

Transparency

Many animals of the open sea, like this Aurelia labiata jellyfish, are largely transparent.

Many marine animals that float near the surface are highly transparent, giving them almost perfect camouflage.[88] However, transparency is difficult for bodies made of materials that have different refractive indices from seawater. Some marine animals such as jellyfish have gelatinous bodies, composed mainly of water; their thick mesogloea is acellular and highly transparent. This conveniently makes them buoyant, but it also makes them large for their muscle mass, so they cannot swim fast, making this form of camouflage a costly trade-off with mobility.[88] Gelatinous planktonic animals are between 50 and 90 percent transparent. A transparency of 50 percent is enough to make an animal invisible to a predator such as cod at a depth of 650 metres (2,130 ft); better transparency is required for invisibility in shallower water, where the light is brighter and predators can see better. For example, a cod can see prey that are 98 percent transparent in optimal lighting in shallow water. Therefore, sufficient transparency for camouflage is more easily achieved in deeper waters.[88]

Glass frogs like Hyalinobatrachium uranoscopum use partial transparency for camouflage in the dim light of the rainforest.

Some tissues such as muscles can be made transparent, provided either they are very thin or organised as regular layers or fibrils that are small compared to the wavelength of visible light. A familiar example is the transparency of the lens of the vertebrate eye, which is made of the protein crystallin, and the vertebrate cornea which is made of the protein collagen.[88] Other structures cannot be made transparent, notably the retinas or equivalent light-absorbing structures of eyes – they must absorb light to be able to function. The camera-type eye of vertebrates and cephalopods must be completely opaque.[88] Finally, some structures are visible for a reason, such as to lure prey. For example, the nematocysts (stinging cells) of the transparent siphonophore Agalma okenii resemble small copepods.[88] Examples of transparent marine animals include a wide variety of larvae, including radiata (coelenterates), siphonophores, salps (floating tunicates), gastropod molluscs, polychaete worms, many shrimplike crustaceans, and fish; whereas the adults of most of these are opaque and pigmented, resembling the seabed or shores where they live.[88][89] Adult comb jellies and jellyfish obey the rule, often being mainly transparent. Cott suggests this follows the more general rule that animals resemble their background: in a transparent medium like seawater, that means being transparent.[89] The small Amazon river fish Microphilypnus amazonicus and the shrimps it associates with, Pseudopalaemon gouldingi, are so transparent as to be "almost invisible"; further, these species appear to select whether to be transparent or more conventionally mottled (disruptively patterned) according to the local background in the environment.[90]

Silvering

The adult herring, Clupea harengus, is a typical silvered fish of medium depths, camouflaged by reflection.
The herring's reflectors are nearly vertical for camouflage from the side.

Where transparency cannot be achieved, it can be imitated effectively by silvering to make an animal's body highly reflective. At medium depths at sea, light comes from above, so a mirror oriented vertically makes animals such as fish invisible from the side. Most fish in the upper ocean such as sardine and herring are camouflaged by silvering.[91]

The marine hatchetfish is extremely flattened laterally, leaving the body just millimetres thick, and the body is so silvery as to resemble aluminium foil. The mirrors consist of microscopic structures similar to those used to provide structural coloration: stacks of between 5 and 10 crystals of guanine spaced about ¼ of a wavelength apart to interfere constructively and achieve nearly 100 per cent reflection. In the deep waters that the hatchetfish lives in, only blue light with a wavelength of 500 nanometres percolates down and needs to be reflected, so mirrors 125 nanometres apart provide good camouflage.[91]

In fish such as the herring which live in shallower water, the mirrors must reflect a mixture of wavelengths, and the fish accordingly has crystal stacks with a range of different spacings. A further complication for fish with bodies that are rounded in cross-section is that the mirrors would be ineffective if laid flat on the skin, as they would fail to reflect horizontally. The overall mirror effect is achieved with many small reflectors, all oriented vertically.[91] Silvering is found in other marine animals as well as fish. The cephalopods, including squid, octopus and cuttlefish, have multi-layer mirrors made of protein rather than guanine.[91]

Mimesis

In mimesis (also called masquerade), the camouflaged object looks like something else which is of no special interest to the observer.[92] Mimesis is common in prey animals, for example when a peppered moth caterpillar mimics a twig, or a grasshopper mimics a dry leaf.[93] It is also found in nest structures; some eusocial wasps, such as Leipomeles dorsata, build a nest envelope in patterns that mimic the leaves surrounding the nest.[94]

Mimesis is also employed by some predators and parasites to lure their prey. For example, a flower mantis mimics a particular kind of flower, such as an orchid.[95] This tactic has occasionally been used in warfare, for example with heavily armed Q-ships disguised as merchant ships.[96][97][98]

The common cuckoo, a brood parasite, provides examples of mimesis both in the adult and in the egg. The female lays her eggs in nests of other, smaller species of bird, one per nest. The female mimics a sparrowhawk. The resemblance is sufficient to make small birds take action to avoid the apparent predator. The female cuckoo then has time to lay her egg in their nest without being seen to do so.[99] The cuckoo's egg itself mimics the eggs of the host species, reducing its chance of being rejected.[100][101]

Motion dazzle

The zebra's bold pattern may induce motion dazzle in observers

Most forms of camouflage are made ineffective by movement: a deer or grasshopper may be highly cryptic when motionless, but instantly seen when it moves. But one method, motion dazzle, requires rapidly moving bold patterns of contrasting stripes.[102] Motion dazzle may degrade predators' ability to estimate the prey's speed and direction accurately, giving the prey an improved chance of escape.[103] Motion dazzle distorts speed perception and is most effective at high speeds; stripes can also distort perception of size (and so, perceived range to the target). As of 2011, motion dazzle had been proposed for military vehicles, but never applied.[102] Since motion dazzle patterns would make animals more difficult to locate accurately when moving, but easier to see when stationary, there would be an evolutionary trade-off between motion dazzle and crypsis.[103]

An animal that is commonly thought to be dazzle-patterned is the zebra. The bold stripes of the zebra have been claimed to be disruptive camouflage,[104] background-blending and countershading.[105][e] After many years in which the purpose of the coloration was disputed,[106] an experimental study by Tim Caro suggested in 2012 that the pattern reduces the attractiveness of stationary models to biting flies such as horseflies and tsetse flies.[107][108] However, a simulation study by Martin How and Johannes Zanker in 2014 suggests that when moving, the stripes may confuse observers, such as mammalian predators and biting insects, by two visual illusions: the wagon-wheel effect, where the perceived motion is inverted, and the barberpole illusion, where the perceived motion is in a wrong direction.[109]

Applications

Military

Before 1800

Roman ships, depicted on a 3rd-century AD sarcophagus

Ship camouflage was occasionally used in ancient times. Philostratus (c. 172–250 AD) wrote in his Imagines that Mediterranean pirate ships could be painted blue-gray for concealment.[110]Vegetius (c. 360–400 AD) says that "Venetian blue" (sea green) was used in the Gallic Wars, when Julius Caesar sent his speculatoria navigia (reconnaissance boats) to gather intelligence along the coast of Britain; the ships were painted entirely in bluish-green wax, with sails, ropes and crew the same colour.[111] There is little evidence of military use of camouflage on land before 1800, but two unusual ceramics show men in Peru's Mochica culture from before 500 AD, hunting birds with blowpipes which are fitted with a kind of shield near the mouth, perhaps to conceal the hunters' hands and faces.[112] Another early source is a 15th-century French manuscript, The Hunting Book of Gaston Phebus, showing a horse pulling a cart which contains a hunter armed with a crossbow under a cover of branches, perhaps serving as a hide for shooting game.[113] Jamaican Maroons are said to have used plant materials as camouflage in the First Maroon War (c. 1655–1740).[114]

19th-century origins

Green-jacketed rifleman firing Baker rifle 1803

The development of military camouflage was driven by the increasing range and accuracy of infantry firearms in the 19th century. In particular the replacement of the inaccurate musket with weapons such as the Baker rifle made personal concealment in battle essential. Two Napoleonic War skirmishing units of the British Army, the 95th Rifle Regiment and the 60th Rifle Regiment, were the first to adopt camouflage in the form of a rifle green jacket, while the Line regiments continued to wear scarlet tunics.[115] A contemporary study in 1800 by the English artist and soldier Charles Hamilton Smith provided evidence that grey uniforms were less visible than green ones at a range of 150 yards.[116]

In the American Civil War, rifle units such as the 1st United States Sharp Shooters (in the Federal army) similarly wore green jackets while other units wore more conspicuous colours.[117] The first British Army unit to adopt khaki uniforms was the Corps of Guides at Peshawar, when Sir Harry Lumsden and his second in command, William Hodson introduced a "drab" uniform in 1848.[118] Hodson wrote that it would be more appropriate for the hot climate, and help make his troops "invisible in a land of dust".[119] Later they improvised by dyeing cloth locally. Other regiments in India soon adopted the khaki uniform, and by 1896 khaki drill uniform was used everywhere outside Europe;[120] by the Second Boer War six years later it was used throughout the British Army.[121]

First World War

Iron observation post camouflaged as a tree by Cubist painter André Mare, 1916

In the First World War, the French army formed a camouflage corps, led by Lucien-Victor Guirand de Scévola,[122][123] employing artists known as camoufleurs to create schemes such as tree observation posts and covers for guns. Other armies soon followed them.[124][125][126] The term camouflage probably comes from camoufler, a Parisian slang term meaning to disguise, and may have been influenced by camouflet, a French term meaning smoke blown in someone's face.[127][128] The English zoologist John Graham Kerr, artist Solomon J. Solomon and the American artist Abbott Thayer led attempts to introduce scientific principles of countershading and disruptive patterning into military camouflage, with limited success.[129][130]

Ship camouflage was introduced in the early 20th century as the range of naval guns increased, with ships painted grey all over.[131][132] In April 1917, when German U-boats were sinking many British ships with torpedoes, the marine artist Norman Wilkinson devised dazzle camouflage, which paradoxically made ships more visible but harder to target.[133] In Wilkinson's own words, dazzle was designed "not for low visibility, but in such a way as to break up her form and thus confuse a submarine officer as to the course on which she was heading".[134]

Second World War

In the Second World War, the zoologist Hugh Cott, a protégé of Kerr, worked to persuade the British army to use more effective camouflage methods, including countershading, but, like Kerr and Thayer in the First World War, with limited success. For example, he painted two rail-mounted coastal guns, one in conventional style, one countershaded. In aerial photographs, the countershaded gun was essentially invisible.[135] The power of aerial observation and attack led every warring nation to camouflage targets of all types. The Soviet Union's Red Army created the comprehensive doctrine of Maskirovka for military deception, including the use of camouflage.[136] For example, during the Battle of Kursk, General Katukov, the commander of the Soviet 1st Tank Army, remarked that the enemy "did not suspect that our well-camouflaged tanks were waiting for him. As we later learned from prisoners, we had managed to move our tanks forward unnoticed". The tanks were concealed in previously prepared defensive emplacements, with only their turrets above ground level.[137] In the air, Second World War fighters were often painted in ground colours above and sky colours below, attempting two different camouflage schemes for observers above and below.[138] Bombers and night fighters were often black,[139] while maritime reconnaissance planes were usually white, to avoid appearing as dark shapes against the sky.[140] For ships, dazzle camouflage was mainly replaced with plain grey in the Second World War, though experimentation with colour schemes continued.[131]

As in the First World War, artists were pressed into service; for example, the surrealist painter Roland Penrose became a lecturer at the newly founded Camouflage Development and Training Centre at Farnham Castle,[141] writing the practical Home Guard Manual of Camouflage.[142] The film-maker Geoffrey Barkas ran the Middle East Command Camouflage Directorate during the 1941–1942 war in the Western Desert, including the successful deception of Operation Bertram. Hugh Cott was chief instructor; the artist camouflage officers, who called themselves camoufleurs, included Steven Sykes and Tony Ayrton.[143][144] In Australia, artists were also prominent in the Sydney Camouflage Group, formed under the chairmanship of Professor William John Dakin, a zoologist from Sydney University. Max Dupain, Sydney Ure Smith, and William Dobell were among the members of the group, which worked at Bankstown Airport, RAAF Base Richmond and Garden Island Dockyard.[145]

After 1945

Camouflage has been used to protect military equipment such as vehicles, guns, ships,[131] aircraft and buildings[146] as well as individual soldiers and their positions.[147] Vehicle camouflage methods begin with paint, which offers at best only limited effectiveness. Other methods for stationary land vehicles include covering with improvised materials such as blankets and vegetation, and erecting nets, screens and soft covers which may suitably reflect, scatter or absorb near infrared and radar waves.[148][149][150] Some military textiles and vehicle camouflage paints also reflect infrared to help provide concealment from night vision devices.[151] After the Second World War, radar made camouflage generally less effective, though coastal boats are sometimes painted like land vehicles.[131] Aircraft camouflage too came to be seen as less important because of radar, and aircraft of different air forces, such as the Royal Air Force's Lightning, were often uncamouflaged.[152]

Many camouflaged textile patterns have been developed to suit the need to match combat clothing to different kinds of terrain (such as woodland, snow, and desert).[153] The design of a pattern effective in all terrains has proved elusive.[154][155][156] The American Universal Camouflage Pattern of 2004 attempted to suit all environments, but was withdrawn after a few years of service.[157] Terrain-specific patterns have sometimes been developed but are ineffective in other terrains.[158] The problem of making a pattern that works at different ranges has been solved with multiscale designs, often with a pixellated appearance and designed digitally, that provide a fractal-like range of patch sizes so they appear disruptively coloured both at close range and at a distance.[159] The first genuinely digital camouflage pattern was the Canadian Disruptive Pattern (CADPAT), issued to the army in 2002, soon followed by the American Marine pattern (MARPAT). A pixellated appearance is not essential for this effect, though it is simpler to design and to print.[160][161]

Hunting

A hide used in field sports

Hunters of game have long made use of camouflage in the form of materials such as animal skins, mud, foliage, and green or brown clothing to enable them to approach wary game animals.[162] Field sports such as driven grouse shooting conceal hunters in hides (also called blinds or shooting butts).[163] Modern hunting clothing makes use of fabrics that provide a disruptive camouflage pattern; for example, in 1986 the hunter Bill Jordan created cryptic clothing for hunters, printed with images of specific kinds of vegetation such as grass and branches.[164]

Civil structures

Cellphone tower disguised as a tree

Camouflage is occasionally used to make built structures less conspicuous: for example, in South Africa, towers carrying cell telephone antennae are sometimes camouflaged as tall trees with plastic branches, in response to "resistance from the community". Since this method is costly (a figure of three times the normal cost is mentioned), alternative forms of camouflage can include using neutral colours or familiar shapes such as cylinders and flagpoles. Conspicuousness can also be reduced by siting masts near, or on, other structures.[165]

Automotive manufacturers often use patterns to disguise upcoming products. This camouflage is designed to obfuscate the vehicle's visual lines, and is used along with padding, covers, and decals. The patterns' purpose is to prevent visual observation (and to a lesser degree photography), that would subsequently enable reproduction of the vehicle's form factors.[166]

Fashion, art and society

1919 dazzle ball costumes
The "dazzle ball" held by the Chelsea Arts Club, 1919

Military camouflage patterns influenced fashion and art from the time of the First World War onwards. Gertrude Stein recalled the cubist artist Pablo Picasso's reaction in around 1915:

I very well remember at the beginning of the war being with Picasso on the boulevard Raspail when the first camouflaged truck passed. It was at night, we had heard of camouflage but we had not seen it and Picasso amazed looked at it and then cried out, yes it is we who made it, that is cubism.

— Gertrude Stein in From Picasso (1938)[167]

In 1919, the attendants of a "dazzle ball", hosted by the Chelsea Arts Club, wore dazzle-patterned black and white clothing. The ball influenced fashion and art via postcards and magazine articles.[168] The Illustrated London News announced:[168][169]

The scheme of decoration for the great fancy dress ball given by the Chelsea Arts Club at the Albert Hall, the other day, was based on the principles of "Dazzle", the method of "camouflage" used during the war in the painting of ships ... The total effect was brilliant and fantastic.

More recently, fashion designers have often used camouflage fabric for its striking designs, its "patterned disorder" and its symbolism.[170] Camouflage clothing can be worn largely for its symbolic significance rather than for fashion, as when, during the late 1960s and early 1970s in the United States, anti-war protestors often ironically wore military clothing during demonstrations against the American involvement in the Vietnam War.[171]

Modern artists such as Ian Hamilton Finlay have used camouflage to reflect on war. His 1973 screenprint of a tank camouflaged in a leaf pattern, Arcadia, is described by the Tate as drawing "an ironic parallel between this idea of a natural paradise and the camouflage patterns on a tank".[172] The title refers to the Utopian Arcadia of poetry and art, and the memento mori Latin phrase Et in Arcadia ego which recurs in Hamilton Finlay's work. In science fiction, Camouflage is a novel about shapeshifting alien beings by Joe Haldeman.[173] The word is used more figuratively in works of literature such as Thaisa Frank's collection of stories of love and loss, A Brief History of Camouflage.[174]

Notes

  1. ^ A letter from Alfred Russel Wallace to Darwin of March 8, 1868 mentioned such colour change: "Would you like to see the specimens of pupæ of butterflies whose colours have changed in accordance with the colour of the surrounding objects? They are very curious, and Mr. T. W. Wood, who bred them, would, I am sure, be delighted to bring them to show you."[5]
  2. ^ Cott explains Beddard's observation as a coincident disruptive pattern.[9]
  3. ^ Before 1860, unpolluted tree trunks were often covered in pale lichens; polluted trunks were bare, and often nearly black.
  4. ^ These distraction markings are sometimes called dazzle markings, but have nothing to do with motion dazzle or wartime dazzle painting.
  5. ^ The belly of the zebra is white, and the dark stripes narrow towards the belly, so the animal is certainly countershaded, but this does not prove that the main function of the stripes is camouflage.

References

  1. ^ a b Aristotle (c. 350 BC). Historia Animalium. IX, 622a: 2–10. Cited in Borrelli, Luciana; Gherardi, Francesca; Fiorito, Graziano (2006). A catalogue of body patterning in Cephalopoda. Firenze University Press. ISBN 978-88-8453-377-7. Abstract
  2. ^ Darwin 1859.
  3. ^ a b Darwin 1859, p. 84.
  4. ^ Poulton 1890, p. 111.
  5. ^ Alfred Russel Wallace (8 March 1868). "Alfred Russel Wallace Letters and Reminiscences By James Marchant". Darwin Online. Retrieved 29 March 2013.
  6. ^ Poulton 1890, p. Fold-out after p. 339.
  7. ^ Beddard 1892, p. 83.
  8. ^ Beddard 1892, p. 87.
  9. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 74–75.
  10. ^ Beddard 1892, p. 122.
  11. ^ Thayer 1909.
  12. ^ Forbes 2009, p. 77.
  13. ^ Thayer 1909, pp. 5, 16.
  14. ^ Rothenberg 2011, pp. 132–133.
  15. ^ Wright, Patrick (23 June 2005). "Cubist Slugs. Review of DPM: Disruptive Pattern Material; An Encyclopedia of Camouflage: Nature – Military – Culture by Roy Behrens". London Review of Books. 27 (12): 16–20.
  16. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 172–173.
  17. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 47–67.
  18. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 174–186.
  19. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 153–155.
  20. ^ "Skin pigmentation provides evidence of convergent melanism in extinct marine reptiles". Nature. 506 (7489): 484–488. February 2014. doi:10.1038/nature12899. PMID 24402224. {{cite journal}}: Unknown parameter |authors= ignored (help)
  21. ^ Troscianko, Jolyon; Wilson-Aggarwal, Jared; Stevens, Martin; Spottiswoode, Claire N. (29 January 2016). "Camouflage predicts survival in ground-nesting birds". Scientific Reports. 6 (1): 19966. doi:10.1038/srep19966. PMC 4731810. PMID 26822039.
  22. ^ a b Forbes 2009, pp. 50–51 and passim.
  23. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 5–19.
  24. ^ Forbes 2009, p. 51.
  25. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 5–6.
  26. ^ Newark 2007, pp. 45–46.
  27. ^ a b c Cott 1940, p. 17.
  28. ^ Still, J. (1996). Collins Wild Guide: Butterflies and Moths. HarperCollins. p. 158. ISBN 978-0-00-220010-3.
  29. ^ Barbosa, A.; Mathger, L. M.; Buresch, K. C.; Kelly, J.; Chubb, C.; Chiao, C.; Hanlon R. T. (2008). "Cuttlefish camouflage: The effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle or disruptive body patterns". Vision Research. 48 (10): 1242–1253. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.011. PMID 18395241.
  30. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 83–91.
  31. ^ Osorio, Daniel; Cuthill, Innes C. "Camouflage and perceptual organization in the animal kingdom" (PDF). Retrieved 25 October 2013.
  32. ^ Stevens, M.; Cuthill, I. C.; Windsor, A. M. M.; Walker, H. J. (7 October 2006). "Disruptive contrast in animal camouflage". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 273 (1600): 2433–2436. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3614. PMC 1634902. PMID 16959632.
  33. ^ Sweet, K. M. (2006). Transportation and Cargo Security: Threats and Solutions. Prentice Hall. p. 219. ISBN 978-0-13-170356-8.
  34. ^ FM 5–20: Camouflage, Basic Principles. U.S. War Department. November 2015 [1944].
  35. ^ Field Manual Headquarters No. 20-3. Department of the Army. 30 August 1999. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  36. ^ Roosevelt, Theodore (1911). "Revealing and concealing coloration in birds and mammals". Bulletin of the American Museum of Natural History. 30 (Article 8): 119–231. Roosevelt attacks Thayer on page 191, arguing that neither zebra nor giraffe are "'adequately obliterated' by countershading or coloration pattern or anything else."
  37. ^ a b c d e Mitchell, G.; Skinner, J. D. (2003). "On the origin, evolution and phylogeny of giraffes Giraffa camelopardalis" (PDF). Transactions of the Royal Society of South Africa. 58 (1): 51–73. doi:10.1080/00359190309519935.
  38. ^ Lev-Yadun, Simcha (2003). "Why do some thorny plants resemble green zebras?". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 224 (4): 483–489. doi:10.1016/s0022-5193(03)00196-6.
  39. ^ Lev-Yadun, Simcha (2006). Teixeira da Silva, J.A. (ed.). Defensive coloration in plants: a review of current ideas about anti-herbivore coloration strategies. Global Science Books. pp. 292–299. ISBN 978-4903313092. {{cite book}}: |work= ignored (help)
  40. ^ Givnish, T. J. (1990). "Leaf Mottling: Relation to Growth Form and Leaf Phenology and Possible Role as Camouflage". Functional Ecology. 4 (4): 463–474. doi:10.2307/2389314. JSTOR 2389314.
  41. ^ a b Sherbrooke, W. C. (2003). Introduction to horned lizards of North America. University of California Press. pp. 117–118. ISBN 978-0-520-22825-2.
  42. ^ a b Cott 1940, pp. 104–105.
  43. ^ U.S. War Department (November 1943). "Principles of Camouflage". Tactical and Technical Trends (37).
  44. ^ Stevens, M.; Merilaita, S. (2009). "Defining disruptive coloration and distinguishing its functions". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 364 (1516): 481–488. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0216. PMC 2674077. PMID 18990673.
  45. ^ Dimitrova, M.; Stobbe, N.; Schaefer, H. M.; Merilaita, S. (2009). "Concealed by conspicuousness: distractive prey markings and backgrounds". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1663): 1905–1910. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0052. PMC 2674505. PMID 19324754.
  46. ^ Wierauch, C. (2006). "Anatomy of disguise: camouflaging structures in nymphs of Some Reduviidae (Heteroptera)". American Museum Novitates. 3542: 1–18. doi:10.1206/0003-0082(2006)3542[1:AODCSI]2.0.CO;2. hdl:2246/5820.
  47. ^ Bates, Mary (10 June 2015). "Natural Bling: 6 Amazing Animals That Decorate Themselves". National Geographic. Retrieved 11 June 2015.
  48. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 102–103.
  49. ^ Cott 1940, p. 360.
  50. ^ Ruxton, Graeme D.; Stevens, Martin (1 June 2015). "The evolutionary ecology of decorating behaviour". Biology Letters. 11 (6): 20150325. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0325. PMC 4528480. PMID 26041868.
  51. ^ a b Cott 1940, p. 141.
  52. ^ "What is a Horned Lizard?". hornedlizards.org. Horned Lizard Conservation Society. Retrieved 14 November 2015.
  53. ^ "Leafy Sea Dragon". WWF. Retrieved 21 December 2011.
  54. ^ Bian, Xue; Elgar, Mark A.; Peters, Richard A. (2016). "The swaying behavior of Extatosoma tiaratum: motion camouflage in a stick insect?". Behavioral Ecology. 27 (1): 83–92. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv125.
  55. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 141–143.
  56. ^ a b Srinivasan, M. V.; Davey, M. (1995). "Strategies for active camouflage of motion". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 259 (1354): 19–25. doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0004.
  57. ^ Hopkin, Michael (5 June 2003). "Dragonfly flight tricks the eye". Nature.com. Retrieved 16 January 2012.
  58. ^ Mizutani, A. K.; Chahl, J. S.; Srinivasan, M. V. (5 June 2003). "Insect behaviour: Motion camouflage in dragonflies". Nature. 65 (423): 604. doi:10.1038/423604a. PMID 12789327.
  59. ^ Glendinning, P (2004). "The mathematics of motion camouflage". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 271 (1538): 477–481. doi:10.1098/rspb.2003.2622. PMC 1691618. PMID 15129957.
  60. ^ Ghose, K.; Horiuchi, T. K.; Krishnaprasad, P.S.; Moss, C. F. (2006). "Echolocating Bats Use a Nearly Time-Optimal Strategy to Intercept Prey". PLoS Biology. 4 (5): e108. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040108. PMC 1436025. PMID 16605303.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  61. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 30–31.
  62. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 52, 236.
  63. ^ Stuart-Fox, Devi; Moussalli, Adnan; Whiting, Martin J. (23 August 2008). "Predator-specific camouflage in chameleons". Biology Letters. 4 (4): 326–9. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0173. PMC 2610148. PMID 18492645.
  64. ^ Wallin, M. (2002). "Nature's Palette" (PDF). Bioscience Explained. 1 (2): 1–12. Retrieved 17 November 2011.
  65. ^ Cott 1940, p. 32.
  66. ^ Cloney, R. A.; Florey, E. (1968). "Ultrastructure of Cephalopod Chromatophore Organs". Zeitschrift für Zellforschung und Mikroskopische Anatomie. 89 (2): 250–280. doi:10.1007/BF00347297. PMID 5700268.
  67. ^ "Day Octopuses, Octopus cyanea". MarineBio Conservation Society. Retrieved 31 January 2013.
  68. ^ a b "Arctic Wildlife". Churchill Polar Bears. 2011. Retrieved 22 December 2011.
  69. ^ Hearn, Brian (20 February 2012). The Status of Arctic Hare (Lepus arcticus bangsii) in Insular Newfoundland (PDF). Newfoundland Labrador Department of Environment and Conservation. p. 7. Retrieved 3 February 2013.
  70. ^ "Tanks test infrared invisibility cloak". BBC News. 5 September 2011. Retrieved 13 June 2012.
  71. ^ "Adaptiv – A Cloak of Invisibility". BAE Systems. 2011. Retrieved 14 November 2015.
  72. ^ "Innovation Adaptiv Car Signature". BAE Systems. 2012. Retrieved 14 November 2015.
  73. ^ Cott 1940, pp. 35–46.
  74. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 72–73.
  75. ^ a b Kiltie, Richard A. (January 1998). "Countershading: Universally deceptive or deceptively universal?". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 3 (1): 21–23. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(88)90079-1. PMID 21227055.
  76. ^ Cott 1940, p. 41.
  77. ^ Ehrlich, Paul R.; Dobkin, David S.; Wheye, Darryl (1988). "The Color of Birds". Stanford University. Retrieved 1 February 2013.
  78. ^ Cott 1940, p. 40.
  79. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 146–150.
  80. ^ Forbes 2009, p. 152.
  81. ^ Barkas 1952, p. 36.
  82. ^ Elias 2011, pp. 57–66.
  83. ^ "Midwater Squid, Abralia veranyi". Smithsonian National Museum of Natural History. Retrieved 28 November 2011.
  84. ^ Young, Richard Edward (October 1983). "Oceanic Bioluminescence: an Overview of General Functions". Bulletin of Marine Science. 33 (4): 829–845.
  85. ^ Douglas, R. H.; Mullineaux, C. W.; Partridge, J. C. (September 2000). "Long-wave sensitivity in deep-sea stomiid dragonfish with far-red bioluminescence: evidence for a dietary origin of the chlorophyll-derived retinal photosensitizer of Malacosteus niger". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 355 (1401): 1269–1272. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0681. PMC 1692851. PMID 11079412.
  86. ^ a b c "Diffused Lighting and its use in the Chaleur Bay". Naval Museum of Quebec. Royal Canadian Navy. Archived from the original on 22 May 2013. Retrieved 3 February 2013.
  87. ^ a b Hambling, David (9 May 2008). "Cloak of Light Makes Drone Invisible?". Wired. Retrieved 17 June 2012.
  88. ^ a b c d e f g Herring 2002, pp. 190–191.
  89. ^ a b Cott 1940, p. 6.
  90. ^ Carvalho, Lucélia Nobre; Zuanon, Jansen; Sazima, Ivan (April–June 2006). "The almost invisible league: crypsis and association between minute fishes and shrimps as a possible defence against visually hunting predators". Neotropical Ichthyology. 4 (2): 219–224. doi:10.1590/S1679-62252006000200008.
  91. ^ a b c d Herring 2002, pp. 192–195.
  92. ^ Gullan, P. J.; Cranston, P. S. (2010). The Insects (4th ed.). John Wiley, Blackwell. pp. 512–513. ISBN 978-1-4443-3036-6.
  93. ^ Forbes 2009, p. 151.
  94. ^ Ross, Kenneth G. (1991). The Social Biology of Wasps. Cornell Press. p. 233. ISBN 978-0-801-49906-7.
  95. ^ Forbes 2009, p. 134.
  96. ^ Beyer, Kenneth M (1999). Q-Ships versus U-Boats: America's Secret Project. Naval Institute Press. ISBN 978-1-55750-044-1.
  97. ^ McMullen, Chris (2001). "Royal Navy 'Q' Ships". Great War Primary Documents Archive. Retrieved 6 March 2012.
  98. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 6–42.
  99. ^ Welbergen, J; Davies, N. B. (2011). "A parasite in wolf's clothing: hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (3): 574–579. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr008.
  100. ^ Brennand, Emma (24 March 2011). "Cuckoo in egg pattern 'arms race'". BBC News. Retrieved 22 August 2011.
  101. ^ Moskát, C; Honza, M. (2002). "European Cuckoo Cuculus canorus parasitism and host's rejection behaviour in a heavily parasitized Great Reed Warbler Acrocephalus arundinaceus population". Ibis. 144 (4): 614–622. doi:10.1046/j.1474-919X.2002.00085.x.
  102. ^ a b Scott-Samuel, N. E.; Baddeley, R.; Palmer, C. E.; Cuthill, Innes C. (June 2011). Burr, David C. (ed.). "Dazzle Camouflage Affects Speed Perception". PLOS One. 6 (6): e20233. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020233. PMC 3105982. PMID 21673797.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  103. ^ a b Stevens, Martin; Searle, W. T. L.; Seymour, J. E.; Marshall, K. L. A.; Ruxton, Graeme D. (25 November 2011). "Motion dazzle and camouflage as distinct anti-predator defenses". BMC Biology. 9: 9–81. doi:10.1186/1741-7007-9-81. PMC 3257203. PMID 22117898.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link)
  104. ^ Cott 1940, p. 94.
  105. ^ Thayer 1909, p. 136.
  106. ^ Caro, Tim (2009). "Contrasting coloration in terrestrial mammals". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 364 (1516): 537–548. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0221. PMC 2674080. PMID 18990666.
  107. ^ Waage, J. K. (1981). "How the zebra got its stripes: biting flies as selective agents in the evolution of zebra colouration". J. Entom. Soc. South Africa. 44: 351–358.
  108. ^ Egri, Ádám; Blahó, Miklós; Kriska ,György; Farkas, Róbert; Gyurkovszky, Mónika; Åkesson,Susanne; Horváth, Gábor (March 2012). "Polarotactic tabanids find striped patterns with brightness and/or polarization modulation least attractive: an advantage of zebra stripes". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 215 (5): 736–745. doi:10.1242/jeb.065540. PMID 22323196.
  109. ^ How, Martin J.; Zanker, Johannes M. (2014). "Motion camouflage induced by zebra stripes". Zoology. 117 (3): 163–170. doi:10.1016/j.zool.2013.10.004. PMID 24368147.
  110. ^ Casson 1995, pp. 211–212.
  111. ^ Casson 1995, p. 235.
  112. ^ Jett, Stephen C. (March 1991). "Further Information on the Geography of the Blowgun and Its Implications for Early Transoceanic Contacts". Annals of the Association of American Geographers. 81 (1): 89–102. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8306.1991.tb01681.x. JSTOR 2563673.
  113. ^ Payne-Gallwey, Ralph (1903). The Crossbow. Longmans, Green. p. 11.
  114. ^ Saunders, Nicholas (2005). The People of the Caribbean: An Encyclopedia of Archaeology and Traditional Culture. ABC-CLIO.
  115. ^ Haythornthwaite, P. (2002). British Rifleman 1797–1815. Osprey Publishing. p. 20. ISBN 978-1841761770.
  116. ^ Newark 2007, p. 43.
  117. ^ "Killers in Green Coats". Weider History Group. 20 February 2008. Retrieved 8 July 2012.
  118. ^ "Khaki Uniform 1848-49: First Introduction by Lumsden and Hodson". Journal of the Society for Army Historical Research. 82 (Winter): 341–347. 2004.
  119. ^ Hodson, W. S. R. (1859). Hodson, George H. (ed.). Twelve Years of a Soldier's Life in India, being extracts from the letters of the late Major WSR Hodson. John W. Parker and Son.
  120. ^ Barthorp, Michael (1988). The British Army on Campaign 1816–1902. Vol. 4. Osprey Publishing. pp. 24–33. ISBN 978-0-85045-849-7.
  121. ^ Chappell, M (2003). The British Army in World War I (1). Osprey Publishing. p. 37. ISBN 978-1-84176-399-6.
  122. ^ Wright, Patrick (23 June 2005). "Cubist Slugs". London Review of Books. 27 (12): 16–20.
  123. ^ Guirand de Scévola, Lucien-Victor (December 1949). "Souvenir de Camouflage (1914–1918)". Revue des Deux Mondes (in French).
  124. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 104–105.
  125. ^ "Art of the First World War: André Mare and Leon Underwood". The Elm at Vermezeele. Memorial-Caen. 1998. Retrieved 8 February 2013.
  126. ^ "Art of the First World War: André Mare". Memorial-Caen. 1998. Retrieved 8 February 2013.
  127. ^ "Camouflage". Online Etymology Dictionary. 2012. Retrieved 8 February 2013.
  128. ^ "Camouflage, n". Oxford English Dictionary. Oxford University Press. Retrieved 8 February 2013.
  129. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 85–89.
  130. ^ For Solomon, see BBC Radio 4 programme "Warpaint: the story of camouflage" by Patrick Wright, August 2002 (repeated Radio 4 Extra, 17 June 2014).
  131. ^ a b c d Sumrall, R. F. (February 1973). "Ship Camouflage (WWII): Deceptive Art". United States Naval Institute Proceedings: 67–81.
  132. ^ Prinzeugen. "Schnellboot: An Illustrated Technical History". Prinz Eugen. Retrieved 5 March 2012.
  133. ^ "Obituary: Mr Norman Wilkinson, Inventor of 'dazzle' painting". The Times. 1 June 1971. p. 12.
  134. ^ Wilkinson, Norman (1969). A Brush with Life. Seeley Service. p. 79.
  135. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 149–150.
  136. ^ Keating, Kenneth C. (1981). "Maskirovka: The Soviet System of Camouflage" (PDF). U.S. Army Russian Institute. Retrieved 8 July 2012.
  137. ^ Clark, Lloyd (2011). Kursk: the greatest battle. Headline Review. p. 278. ISBN 978-0-7553-3639-5.
  138. ^ Shaw, Robert L. (1985). Fighter Combat: Tactics and Maneuvering. Naval Institute Press. ISBN 978-0-87021-059-4.
  139. ^ Stephenson, Hubert Kirk (1948). Applied Physics, pp. 200, 258. Volume 6 of Science in World War II; Office of Scientific Research and Development. Editors: Chauncey Guy Suits and George Russell Harrison. Little, Brown.
  140. ^ Tinbergen, Niko (1953). The Herring Gull's World. Collins. p. 14. ISBN 978-0-00-219444-0. white has proved to be the most efficient concealing coloration for aircraft on anti-submarine patrol
  141. ^ "World War II". Farnham Castle. Retrieved 8 February 2013.
  142. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 151–152.
  143. ^ Barkas 1952, pp. 154, 186–188.
  144. ^ Forbes 2009, pp. 156–166.
  145. ^ Mellor, D P, , Australia in the War of 1939–1945. Series 4 – Civil, Volume 5 The Role of Science and Industry, Australian War Memorial, Canberra, 1968, page 538ff
  146. ^ "Concealment, Camouflage, and Deception" (PDF). Smithsonian. pp. 1–4. Retrieved 16 June 2012.
  147. ^ "FM 21–75". Chapter 1: Cover, Concealment, and Camouflage. Department of the Army. Retrieved 16 June 2012.
  148. ^ "FM 21-305/AFMAN 24-306" (PDF). Chapter 20: Vehicle Camouflage And Nuclear, Biological, And Chemical Operations. Department of the Army. pp. 1–9. Retrieved 16 June 2012.
  149. ^ "5–103". Appendix D: Camouflage. Department of the Army. Retrieved 17 June 2012.
  150. ^ "SSZ Camouflage". Military Suppliers & News. 2012. Retrieved 17 June 2012.
  151. ^ Jukkola, E. E.; Cohen, R. (1946). "Color Stability of Olive Drab Infrared-Reflecting Camouflage Finishes". Industrial & Engineering Chemistry. 38 (9): 927–930. doi:10.1021/ie50441a019.
  152. ^ Richardson, Doug (2001). Stealth Warplanes: Deception, Evasion, and Concealment in the Air. MBI Publishing, Zenith Press. ISBN 978-0-7603-1051-9.
  153. ^ Pfanner, Toni (March 2004). "Military uniforms and the law of war" (PDF). IRRC. 86 (853): 99–100. doi:10.1017/s1560775500180113.
  154. ^ FM 21–76 US Army Survival Manual. Department of the Army. Retrieved 8 January 2013.
  155. ^ Photosimulation Camouflage Detection Test. U.S. Army Natick Soldier Research, Development and Engineering Center. 2009. p. 27. Retrieved 5 October 2012.
  156. ^ Brayley, Martin J (2009). Camouflage uniforms: international combat dress 1940–2010. Crowood. ISBN 978-1-84797-137-1.
  157. ^ Freedberg, S. J. Jr. (25 June 2012). "Army drops universal camouflage after spending billions". AOL Defence. Archived from the original on 31 August 2012. Retrieved 27 September 2012. {{cite news}}: Unknown parameter |dead-url= ignored (|url-status= suggested) (help)
  158. ^ Davies, W. "Berlin Brigade Urban Paint Scheme". Newsletter. Ex-Military Land Rover Association. Retrieved 25 September 2012.
  159. ^ Craemer, Guy. "Dual Texture – U.S. Army digital camouflage". United Dynamics. Retrieved 27 September 2012.
  160. ^ Gye, H. (25 June 2012). "How U.S. Army spent $5 billion on 'failed' pixel camouflage". Daily Mail. Retrieved 21 November 2012.
  161. ^ Engber, D. (5 July 2012) [2007]. "Lost in the Wilderness, the military's misadventures in pixellated camouflage". Slate. Retrieved 27 September 2012.
  162. ^ Newark 2007, p. 38.
  163. ^ Blakeley, Peter F. (2012). Wingshooting. Stackpole Books. pp. 116, 125. ISBN 978-0-8117-0566-0.
  164. ^ Newark 2007, pp. 48, 50.
  165. ^ du Plessis, A. (3 July 2002). Telecommunication Mast Management Guidelines For The City of Tshwane. City of Tshwane Metropolitan Municipality.
  166. ^ "The secrets behind all that camouflage". Automotive News. 12 May 2015. Retrieved 28 July 2015.
  167. ^ "Picasso". Scribners. 1939. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |authors= ignored (help)
  168. ^ a b Forbes 2009, p. 100.
  169. ^ "The Great Dazzle Ball at the Albert Hall: The Shower of Bomb Balloons and Some Typical Costumes". Illustrated London News. No. 154. 22 March 1919. pp. 414–415.{{cite news}}: CS1 maint: location (link)
  170. ^ "Love and War: The Weaponized Woman". John Galliano for Christian Dior, silk camouflage evening dress. The Museum at FIT. 9 September – 16 December 2006. Archived from the original on 12 December 2012. Retrieved 1 December 2011. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |dead-url= ignored (|url-status= suggested) (help)
  171. ^ "Camouflage: The Exhibition". Canadian War Museum. 5 June 2009. Retrieved 14 November 2015.
  172. ^ "Ian Hamilton Finlay: Arcadia (collaboration with George Oliver)". Arcadia, 1973. Tate. July 2008. Retrieved 11 May 2012.
  173. ^ Haldeman, Joe (2004). Camouflage. Ace Books. ISBN 978-0-441-01161-2.
  174. ^ Frank, Thaisa (1992). A Brief History of Camouflage. Black Sparrow Press. ISBN 978-0-87685-857-8.

Bibliography

Camouflage in nature

Early research
General reading

Military camouflage

Further reading

For children
  • Kalman, Bobbie; Crossingham, John (2001). What are Camouflage and Mimicry?. Crabtree Publishing. ISBN 978-0-86505-962-7. (ages 4–8)
  • Mettler, Rene (2001). Animal Camouflage. First Discovery series. Moonlight Publishing. ISBN 978-1-85103-298-3. (ages 4–8)