Empathy
Part of a series on |
Emotions |
---|
Empathy is the capacity to understand or feel what another being (a human or non-human animal) is experiencing from within the other being's frame of reference, i.e., the capacity to place oneself in another's position.[1] There are many definitions for empathy which encompass a broad range of emotional states. Types of empathy include cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and somatic empathy.[2] In the development of human empathy, individual differences appear, ranging from no apparent empathic ability, or empathy which is harmful to self or others, to well-balanced empathy, including the ability to distinguish between self and other.[3] Various theories and aspects of empathy have been researched, including empathy within nonhuman animals.
Etymology
The English word empathy is derived from the Ancient Greek word ἐμπάθεια (empatheia, meaning "physical affection or passion"). This, in turn, comes from ἐν (en, "in, at") and πάθος (pathos, "passion" or "suffering").[4] The term was adapted by Hermann Lotze and Robert Vischer to create the German word Einfühlung ("feeling into"), which was translated by Edward B. Titchener into the English term empathy.[5][6]
Alexithymia is a word used to describe a deficiency in understanding, processing or describing emotions in oneself as opposed to in others.[7] This term comes from the negation of two Ancient Greek words: λέξις (lexis, meaning "speech") and θυμός (thymos, meaning "the soul, as the seat of emotion, feeling and thought"). This combined word is then modified by an alpha privative (prefix a-; that is, "not"). Thus alexithymia literally means "not having speech for your emotions".
Definitions
Empathy has many different definitions that encompass a broad range of emotional states, including caring for other people and having a desire to help them; experiencing emotions that match another person's emotions; discerning what another person is thinking or feeling;[8] and making less distinct the differences between the self and the other.[9]
It also is the ability to feel and share another person’s emotions. Some believe that empathy involves the ability to match another’s emotions, while others believe that empathy involves being tenderhearted toward another person.[10] Compassion and sympathy are two terms that many associate with empathy, but all three of these terms are unique. Compassion is an emotion we feel when others are in need, which motivates us to help them. Sympathy is a feeling of care and understanding for someone in need. It can also be understood as having the separateness of defining oneself and another blur.[11]
In the field of positive psychology, empathy has also been compared with altruism and egotism. Altruism is behavior that is aimed at benefitting another person, while egotism is a behavior that is acted out for personal gain. Sometimes, when someone is feeling empathetic towards another, acts of altruism occur. However, many question whether or not these acts of altruism are motivated by egotistical gains. According to positive psychologists, people can be adequately moved by their empathies to be altruistic.[10] [12]
Since empathy involves understanding the emotional states of other people, the way it is characterized is derivative of the way emotions themselves are characterized. If, for example, emotions are taken to be centrally characterized by bodily feelings, then grasping the bodily feelings of another will be central to empathy. On the other hand, if emotions are more centrally characterized by a combination of beliefs and desires, then grasping these beliefs and desires will be more essential to empathy. The ability to imagine oneself as another person is a sophisticated imaginative process. However, the basic capacity to recognize emotions is probably innate[13] and may be achieved unconsciously. Yet it can be trained[14] and achieved with various degrees of intensity or accuracy.
Empathy necessarily has a "more or less" quality. The paradigm case of an empathic interaction, however, involves a person communicating an accurate recognition of the significance of another person's ongoing intentional actions, associated emotional states, and personal characteristics in a manner that the recognized person can tolerate. Recognitions that are both accurate and tolerable are central features of empathy.[15][16]
The human capacity to recognize the bodily feelings of another is related to one's imitative capacities and seems to be grounded in an innate capacity to associate the bodily movements and facial expressions one sees in another with the proprioceptive feelings of producing those corresponding movements or expressions oneself.[17] Humans seem to make the same immediate connection between the tone of voice and other vocal expressions and inner feeling.
Empathy is distinct from sympathy, pity, and emotional contagion.[18] Sympathy or empathic concern is the feeling of compassion or concern for another, the wish to see them better off or happier. Pity is feeling that another is in trouble and in need of help as they cannot fix their problems themselves, often described as "feeling sorry" for someone. Emotional contagion is when a person (especially an infant or a member of a mob) imitatively "catches" the emotions that others are showing without necessarily recognizing this is happening.[19]
Gender differences
The literature commonly indicates that females are more empathic than males. On average, female subjects score higher than males on the Empathy Quotient (EQ), while males tend to score higher on the Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Both males and females with autistic spectrum disorders usually score lower on the EQ and SQ (see below for more detail on autism and empathy).[20] However, a series of studies, using a variety of neurophysiological measures, including MEG,[21] spinal reflex excitability,[22] electroencephalography[23][24] and N400 paradigm[25] have documented the presence of a gender difference in the human mirror neuron system, with female participants exhibiting stronger motor resonance than male participants. In addition, these aforementioned studies found that female participants scored higher on empathy self-report dispositional measures and that these measures positively correlated with the physiological response. Other studies show no significant difference, and suggests gender differences are the result of motivational differences.[26][27]
A review published in the journal Neuropsychologia found that women are better at recognizing facial effects, expression processing and emotions in general.[28] Men were only better at recognizing specific behavior which includes anger, aggression and threatening cues.[28] A 2006 meta-analysis by researcher Rena A Kirkland in the journal North American Journal of Psychology found significant sex differences favoring females in "Reading of the mind" test. "Reading of the mind" test is an advanced ability measure of cognitive empathy in which Kirkland's analysis involved 259 studies across 10 countries.[29] Another 2014 meta-analysis in the journal of Cognition and Emotion, found overall female advantage in non-verbal emotional recognition across 215 samples.[30]
Using fMRI, neuroscientist Tania Singer showed that empathy-related neural responses are significantly lower in males when observing an "unfair" person experiencing pain.[31] An analysis from the journal of Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews also found that there are sex differences in empathy from birth, growing larger with age and which remains consistent and stable across lifespan.[32] Females were found to have higher empathy than males, while children with higher empathy regardless of gender continue to be higher in empathy throughout development.[32] Further analysis of brain tools such as event related potentials found that females who saw human suffering had higher ERP waveforms than males.[32] Another investigation with similar brain tools such as N400 amplitudes found higher N400 in females in response to social situations which positively correlated with self-reported empathy.[32] Structural fMRI studies also found females to have larger grey matter volumes in posterior inferior frontal and anterior inferior parietal cortex areas which are correlated with mirror neurons in fMRI literature.[32] Females were also found to have stronger link between emotional and cognitive empathy.[32] The researchers found that the stability of these sex differences in development are unlikely to be explained by any environment influences but rather might have some roots in human evolution and inheritance.[32]
Throughout prehistory, females nurtured and were the primary caretakers of children, so this might have led to an evolved neurological adaptation for women to be more aware and responsive to non-verbal expressions. According to the Primary Caretaker Hypothesis, prehistoric males did not have the same selective pressure as primary caretakers; so therefore this might explain modern day sex differences in emotion recognition and empathy.[32]
Major theories and empirical findings
Research investigating the social response to natural disasters looked at the characteristics associated with individuals who help victims. Researchers found that cognitive empathy, rather than emotional empathy, predicted helping behavior towards victims.[33] Others have posited that taking on the perspectives of others (cognitive empathy) allows these individuals to better empathize with victims without as much discomfort, whereas sharing the emotions of the victims (emotional empathy) can cause emotional distress, helplessness, victim-blaming, and ultimately can lead to avoidance rather than helping.[34]
Yet, despite this evidence for empathy-induced altruistic motivation, egoistic explanations may still be possible. For example, one alternative explanation for the problem-specific helping pattern may be that the sequence of events in the same problem condition first made subjects sad when they empathized with the problem and then maintained or enhanced subjects’ sadness when they were later exposed to the same plight. Consequently, the negative state relief model would predict substantial helping among imagine-set subjects in the same condition, which is what occurred. An intriguing question arises from such findings concerning whether it is possible to have mixed motivations for helping. If this is the case, then simultaneous egoistic and altruistic motivations would occur. This would allow for a stronger sadness-based motivation to obscure the effects of an empathic concern-based altruistic motivation. The observed study would then have sadness as less intense than more salient altruistic motivation. Consequently, relative strengths of different emotional reactions, systematically related to the need situation, may moderate the predominance of egoistic or altruistic motivation (Dovidio, 1990). But it has been shown that researchers in this area who have used very similar procedures sometimes obtain apparently contradictory results. Superficial procedural differences such as precisely when a manipulation is introduced could also lead to divergent results and conclusions. It is therefore vital for any future research to move toward even greater standardization of measurement. Thus, an important step in solving the current theoretical debate concerning the existence of altruism may involve reaching common methodological ground.[35]
Contemporary neuroscience has allowed us to understand the neural basis of the human mind’s ability to understand and process emotion. Studies today enable us to see the activation of mirror neurons and attempt to explain the basic processes of empathy. By isolating these mirror neurons and measuring the neural basis for human mind reading and emotion sharing abilities,[36] science has come one step closer to finding the reason for reactions like empathy. Neuroscientists have already discovered that people scoring high on empathy tests have especially busy mirror neuron systems in their brains (Dr. Christian Keysers). [citation needed] Empathy is a spontaneous sharing of affect, provoked by witnessing and sympathizing with another’s emotional state. In a way we mirror or mimic the emotional response that we would expect to feel in that condition or context, much like sympathy. Unlike personal distress, empathy is not characterized by aversion to another’s emotional response. Additionally, empathizing with someone requires a distinctly sympathetic reaction where personal distress demands avoidance of distressing matters. This distinction is vital because empathy is associated with the moral emotion sympathy, or empathetic concern, and consequently also prosocial or altruistic action.[36] Empathy leads to sympathy by definition unlike the over-aroused emotional response that turns into personal distress and causes a turning-away from another’s distress.
In empathy we feel what we believe are the emotions of another, which makes it both affective and cognitive by most psychologists.[8] In this sense, arousal and empathy promote prosocial behavior as we accommodate each other to feel similar emotions. For social beings, negotiating interpersonal decisions is as important to survival as being able navigate the physical landscape.[37] Emotions motivate individual behavior that aids in solving communal challenges as well as guiding group decisions about social exchange. Additionally, recent research has shown individuals who report regular experiences of gratitude engage more frequently in prosocial behaviors. Positive emotions like empathy or gratitude are linked to a more positive continual state and these people are far more likely to help others than those not experiencing a positive emotional state.[38] Thus, empathy’s influence extends beyond relating to other’s emotions, it correlates with an increased positive state and likeliness to aid others. Measures of empathy show that mirror neurons are activated during arousal of sympathetic responses and prolonged activation shows increased probability to help others.
Another growing focus of investigation is how empathy manifests in education between teachers and learners.[39] Although there is general agreement that empathy is essential in educational settings, research has found that it is difficult to develop empathy in trainee teachers.[40] According to one theory, there are seven components involved in the effectiveness of intercultural communication; empathy was found to be one of the seven. This theory also states that empathy is learnable. However, research also shows that it is more difficult to empathize when there are differences between people including status, culture, religion, language, skin colour, gender, age and so on.[40]
In order to achieve intercultural empathy, psychologists have employed empathy training. One study hypothesized that empathy training would increase the measured level of relational empathy among the individuals in the experimental group when compared to the control group.[41] The study also hypothesized that empathy training would increase communication among the experimental group, and that perceived satisfaction with group dialogue would also increase among the experimental group. To test this, the experimenters used the Hogan Empathy Scale, the Barrett-Lennard Relationship Inventory, and questionnaires. Using these measures, the study found that empathy training was not successful in increasing relational empathy. Also, communication and satisfaction among groups did not increase as a result of the empathy training. While there didn’t seem to be a clear relationship between empathy and relational empathy training, the study did report that “relational empathy training appeared to foster greater expectations for a deep dialogic process resulting in treatment differences in perceived depth of communication”.
The environment has been another interesting topic of study. Many theorize that environmental factors, such as parenting style and relationships, play a significant role in the development of empathy in children. Empathy promotes pro social relationships, helps mediate aggression, and allows us to relate to others, all of which make empathy an important emotion among children.
A study done by Caroline Tisot looked at how a variety of environmental factors affected the development of empathy in young children. Parenting style, parent empathy, and prior social experiences were looked at. The children participating in the study were asked to complete an affective empathy measure, while the children’s parents completed the Parenting Practices Questionnaire, which assesses parenting style, and the Balanced Emotional Empathy scale.
This study found that a few parenting practices – as opposed to parenting style as a whole – contributed to the development of empathy in children. These practices include encouraging the child to imagine the perspectives of others and teaching the child to reflect on his or her own feelings. The results also show that the development of empathy varied based on the gender of the child and parent. Paternal warmth was found to be significantly important, and was positively related to empathy within children, especially in boys. Interestingly, however, maternal warmth was negatively related to empathy within children, especially in girls.[42]
Applications
The empathy-altruism relationship also has broad and practical implications. Given the power of empathic feelings to evoke altruistic motivation, people may unfortunately learn to suppress or avoid these feelings. This numbing or even loss of the capacity to feel empathy for clients may be a factor in the experience of burnout among case workers in helping professions. Awareness of this impending futile effort—such as nurses caring for terminal patients or pedestrians walking by the homeless—may make individuals try to avoid feelings of empathy in order to avoid the resulting altruistic motivation. Promoting an understanding about the mechanisms by which altruistic behavior is driven, whether it’s from minimizing sadness or the arousal of mirror neurons, allows people to better cognitively control their actions. However, empathy-induced altruism may not always produce prosocial effects. It could lead one to increase the welfare of those for whom empathy is felt at the expense of other potential prosocial goals, thus inducing a type of bias. Researchers suggest that individuals are willing to act against the greater collective good or to violate their own moral principles of fairness and justice if doing so will benefit a person for whom empathy is felt.[43]
On a more positive note, empathically aroused individuals may focus on the long-term welfare rather than just the short-term of those in need. Empathy-based socialization is very different from current practices directed toward inhibition of egoistic impulses through shaping, modeling and internalized guilt. Therapeutic programs built around facilitating altruistic impulses by encouraging perspective taking and empathic feelings might enable individuals to develop more satisfactory interpersonal relations, especially in the long-term. At a societal level, experiments have indicated that empathy-induced altruism can be used to improve attitudes toward stigmatized groups, even used to improve racial attitudes, actions toward people with AIDS, the homeless and even convicts. Such resulting altruism has also been found to increase cooperation in competitive situations.[44]
Types
Affective, cognitive, and somatic empathy
Empathy can be divided into three major components:[45]
- Affective empathy, also called emotional empathy:,[46] is the capacity to respond with an appropriate emotion to another's mental states.[45] Our ability to empathize emotionally is based on emotional contagion:[46] being affected by another's emotional or arousal state.[47]
- Cognitive empathy: the capacity to understand another's perspective or mental state.[20][45][48] The terms cognitive empathy and theory of mind are often used synonymously, but due to a lack of studies comparing theory of mind with types of empathy, it is unclear whether these are equivalent.[49]
- Somatic empathy is physical reaction, probably based on mirror neuron responses, in the somatic nervous system.[2]
Although science has not yet agreed upon a precise definition of these constructs, there is consensus about this distinction.[50]
Affective empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[45][51]
- Empathic concern: sympathy and compassion for others in response to their suffering.[45][52][53]
- Personal distress: self-centered feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to another's suffering.[45][52][53] There is no consensus regarding whether personal distress is a basic form of empathy or instead does not constitute empathy.[52] There may be a developmental aspect to this subdivision. Infants respond to the distress of others by getting distressed themselves; only when they are 2 years old do they start to respond in other-oriented ways, trying to help, comfort and share.[52]
Cognitive empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[45][51]
- Perspective taking: the tendency to spontaneously adopt others' psychological perspectives.[45]
- Fantasy: the tendency to identify with fictional characters.[45]
Distribution
A difference in distribution between affective and cognitive empathy has been observed in various conditions. Psychopathy and narcissism have been associated with impairments in affective but not cognitive empathy, whereas bipolar disorder and borderline traits have been associated with deficits in cognitive but not affective empathy.[50] Autism spectrum disorders have been associated with various combinations, including deficits in cognitive empathy as well as deficits in both cognitive and affective empathy.[45][46][50][52][54][55] Schizophrenia, too, has been associated with deficits in both types of empathy.[56] However, even in people without conditions such as these, the balance between affective and cognitive empathy varies.[50]
A meta-analysis of recent fMRI studies of empathy confirmed that different brain areas are activated during affective–perceptual empathy and cognitive–evaluative empathy.[57] Also, a study with patients with different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.[46] Specifically, the inferior frontal gyrus appears to be responsible for emotional empathy, and the ventromedial prefrontal gyrus seems to mediate cognitive empathy.[46]
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is the only published measurement tool to date that accounts for a multi-dimensional assessment of empathy. It comprises a self-report questionnaire of 28 items, divided into four 7-item scales covering the above subdivisions of affective and cognitive empathy.[45][51]
Development
By the age of two years, children normally begin to display the fundamental behaviors of empathy by having an emotional response that corresponds with another person's emotional state.[58] Even earlier, at one year of age, infants have some rudiments of empathy, in the sense that they understand that, just like their own actions, other people's actions have goals.[59][60][61] Sometimes, toddlers will comfort others or show concern for them at as early an age as two. Also during the second year, toddlers will play games of falsehood or "pretend" in an effort to fool others, and this requires that the child know what others believe before he or she can manipulate those beliefs.[62] In order to develop these traits, it is essential to expose your child to face-to-face interactions and opportunities and lead them away from a sedentary lifestyle.
According to researchers at the University of Chicago who used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), children between the ages of 7 and 12 years appear to be naturally inclined to feel empathy for others in pain. Their findings[63] are consistent with previous fMRI studies of pain empathy with adults. The research also found additional aspects of the brain were activated when youngsters saw another person intentionally hurt by another individual, including regions involved in moral reasoning.[64]
Despite being able to show some signs of empathy, including attempting to comfort a crying baby, from as early as 18 months to two years, most children do not show a fully fledged theory of mind until around the age of four.[65] Theory of mind involves the ability to understand that other people may have beliefs that are different from one's own, and is thought to involve the cognitive component of empathy.[20] Children usually become capable of passing "false belief" tasks, considered to be a test for a theory of mind, around the age of four. Individuals with autism often find using a theory of mind very difficult (e.g. Baron-Cohen, Leslie & Frith, 1988; the Sally-Anne test).
Empathetic maturity is a cognitive structural theory developed at the Yale University School of Nursing and addresses how adults conceive or understand the personhood of patients. The theory, first applied to nurses and since applied to other professions, postulates three levels that have the properties of cognitive structures. The third and highest level is held to be a meta-ethical theory of the moral structure of care. Those adults operating with level-III understanding synthesize systems of justice and care-based ethics.[66]
Individual differences
Empathy in the broadest sense refers to a reaction of one individual to another’s emotional state. Recent years have seen increased movement toward the idea that empathy occurs from motor neuron imitation. But, how do we account for individual differences in empathy? It cannot be said that empathy is a single unipolar construct but rather a set of constructs. In essence, not every individual responds equally and uniformly the same to various circumstances. The Empathic Concern scale assesses “other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern and the Personal Distress scale measures “self-oriented” feelings of personal anxiety and unease. The combination of these scales helps reveal those that might not be classified as empathetic and expands the narrow definition of empathy. Using this approach we can enlarge the basis of what it means to possess empathetic qualities and create a multi-faceted definition.[67]
Behavioral and neuroimaging research show that two underlying facets of the personality dimensions Extraversion and Agreeableness (the Warmth-Altruistic personality profile) are associated with empathic accuracy and increased brain activity in two brain regions important for empathic processing (medial prefrontal cortex and temporoparietal junction).[68]
Neurological basis
Research in recent years has focused on possible brain processes underlying the experience of empathy. For instance, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has been employed to investigate the functional anatomy of empathy.[69][70] These studies have shown that observing another person's emotional state activates parts of the neuronal network involved in processing that same state in oneself, whether it is disgust,[71] touch,[72][73] or pain.[74][75][76][77] The study of the neural underpinnings of empathy has received increased interest following the target paper published by Preston and Frans de Waal,[78] following the discovery of mirror neurons in monkeys that fire both when the creature watches another perform an action as well as when they themselves perform it.
In their paper, they argue that attended perception of the object's state automatically activates neural representations, and that this activation automatically primes or generates the associated autonomic and somatic responses (idea of perception-action-coupling[79]), unless inhibited. This mechanism is similar to the common coding theory between perception and action. Another recent study provides evidence of separate neural pathways activating reciprocal suppression in different regions of the brain associated with the performance of "social" and "mechanical" tasks. These findings suggest that the cognition associated with reasoning about the "state of another person's mind" and "causal/mechanical properties of inanimate objects" are neurally suppressed from occurring at the same time.[80][81]
A recent meta-analysis of 40 fMRI studies found that affective empathy is correlated with increased activity in the insula while cognitive empathy is correlated with activity in the mid cingulate cortex and adjacent dorsomedial prefrontal cortex.[82]
It has been suggested that mirroring-behavior in motor neurons during empathy may help duplicate feelings.[83] Such sympathetic action may afford access to sympathetic feelings for another and, perhaps, trigger emotions of kindness, forgiveness.[84]
Empathic anger and distress
Anger
Empathic anger is an emotion, a form of empathic distress.[85] Empathic anger is felt in a situation where someone else is being hurt by another person or thing. It is possible to see this form of anger as a pro-social emotion.[citation needed]
Empathic anger has direct effects on both helping and punishing desires. Empathic anger can be divided into two sub-categories: trait empathic anger and state empathic anger.[86]
The relationship between empathy and anger response towards another person has also been investigated, with two studies basically finding that the higher a person's perspective taking ability, the less angry they were in response to a provocation. Empathic concern did not, however, significantly predict anger response, and higher personal distress was associated with increased anger.[87][88]
Distress
Empathic distress is feeling the perceived pain of another person. This feeling can be transformed into empathic anger, feelings of injustice, or guilt. These emotions can be perceived as pro-social, and some say they can be seen as motives for moral behavior.[85]
Atypical response
Atypical empathic responses have been associated with autism and particular personality disorders such as psychopathy, borderline, narcissistic, and schizoid personality disorders; conduct disorder;[89] schizophrenia; bipolar disorder;[50] and depersonalization.[90]
Autism
The interaction between empathy and autism is a complex and ongoing field of research. Several different factors are proposed to be at play. A study of high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorders found an increased prevalence of alexithymia,[91] a personality construct characterized by the inability to recognize and articulate emotional arousal in oneself or others.[91][92][93] Based on fMRI studies, alexithymia is responsible for a lack of empathy.[94] The lack of empathic attunement inherent to alexithymic states may reduce quality[95] and satisfaction[96] of relationships. Recently, a study has shown that high-functioning adults with autism appear to have a range of responses to music similar to that of neurotypical individuals, including the deliberate use of music for mood management. Clinical treatment of alexithymia could involve using a simple associative learning process between musically induced emotions and their cognitive correlates.[97] A study has suggested that the empathy deficits associated with the autism spectrum may be due to significant comorbidity between alexithymia and autism spectrum conditions rather than a result of social impairment.[98]
One study found that, relative to typically developing children, high-functioning children with autism showed reduced mirror neuron activity in the brain's inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) while imitating and observing emotional expressions.[99] EEG evidence revealed that there was significantly greater mu suppression in the sensorimotor cortex of autistic individuals. Activity in this area was inversely related to symptom severity in the social domain, suggesting that a dysfunctional mirror neuron system may underlie social and communication deficits observed in autism, including impaired theory of mind and empathy.[100] The mirror neuron system is essential for emotional empathy.[46]
Previous studies have suggested that autistic individuals have impaired theory of mind. Theory of mind is the ability to understand the perspectives of others.[45] The terms cognitive empathy and theory of mind are often used synonymously, but due to a lack of studies comparing theory of mind with types of empathy, it is unclear whether these are equivalent.[45] Theory of mind relies on structures of the temporal lobe and the pre-frontal cortex, and empathy, i.e. the ability to share the feelings of others, relies on the sensorimotor cortices as well as limbic and para-limbic structures.[citation needed] The lack of clear distinctions between theory of mind and empathy may have resulted in an incomplete understanding of the empathic abilities of those with Asperger syndrome; many reports on the empathic deficits of individuals with Asperger syndrome are actually based on impairments in theory of mind.[45][101][102]
Studies have found that individuals on the autism spectrum self-report lower levels of empathic concern, show less or absent comforting responses toward someone who is suffering, and report equal or higher levels of personal distress compared to controls.[52] The combination of reduced empathic concern and increased personal distress may lead to the overall reduction of empathy in those on the autism spectrum.[52] Professor Simon Baron-Cohen suggests that those with classic autism often lack both cognitive and affective empathy.[55] Research also suggests that people with Asperger syndrome may have problems understanding others' perspectives in terms of theory of mind, but on average demonstrate equal empathic concern as and higher personal distress than controls.[45] The generally heightened personal distress in those with autism spectrum conditions has been offered as an explanation to the claim that at least some people with autism would appear to have heightened emotional empathy,[52][54] although emotional empathy depends on mirror neuron activity, which (as described previously) has been found to be reduced in those with autism, and empathy in people on the autism spectrum is generally reduced.[46][52]
The empathizing–systemizing theory (E-S) suggests that people may be classified on the basis of their capabilities along two independent dimensions, empathizing (E) and systemizing (S). These capabilities may be inferred through tests that measure someone's Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Five different "brain types" can be observed among the population based on the scores, which should correlate with differences at the neural level. In the E-S theory, autism and Asperger syndrome are associated with below-average empathy and average or above-average systemizing. The E-S theory has been extended into the Extreme Male Brain theory, which suggests that people with an autism spectrum condition are more likely to have an "Extreme Type S" brain type, corresponding with above-average systemizing but challenged empathy (see the next section).[103]
It has been shown that autistic males are generally less empathetic than autistic females.[103][104] The Extreme Male Brain (EMB) theory proposes that individuals on the autistic spectrum are characterized by impairments in empathy due to sex differences in the brain: specifically, people with autism spectrum conditions show an exaggerated male profile. A study showed that some aspects of autistic neuroanatomy seem to be extremes of typical male neuroanatomy, which may be influenced by elevated levels of fetal testosterone rather than gender itself.[103][105][106] Another study involving brain scans of 120 men and women suggested that autism affects male and female brains differently; females with autism had brains that appeared to be closer to those of non-autistic males than females, yet the same kind of difference was not observed in males with autism.[107]
Psychopathy
Psychopathy is a personality disorder partly characterized by antisocial and aggressive behaviors, as well as emotional and interpersonal deficits including shallow emotions and a lack of remorse and empathy.[108][109] The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) and International Classification of Diseases (ICD) list antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and dissocial personality disorder, stating that these have been referred to or include what is referred to as psychopathy.[110][111][112][113][114]
A large body of research suggests that psychopathy is associated with atypical responses to distress cues (e.g. facial and vocal expressions of fear and sadness), including decreased activation of the fusiform and extrastriate cortical regions, which may partly account for impaired recognition of and reduced autonomic responsiveness to expressions of fear, and impairments of empathy.[115][116][117][118][119] Studies on children with psychopathic tendencies have also shown such associations.[120][121][122] The underlying biological surfaces for processing expressions of happiness are functionally intact in psychopaths, although less responsive than those of controls.[119][120][121][122] The neuroimaging literature is unclear as to whether deficits are specific to particular emotions such as fear. Some recent fMRI studies have reported that emotion perception deficits in psychopathy are pervasive across emotions (positives and negatives).[123][124]
A recent study on psychopaths found that under certain circumstances, they could willfully empathize with others, and that their empathic reaction initiated the same way it does for controls. Psychopathic criminals were brain-scanned while watching videos of a person harming another individual. The psychopaths' empathic reaction initiated the same way it did for controls when they were instructed to empathize with the harmed individual, and the area of the brain relating to pain was activated when the psychopaths were asked to imagine how the harmed individual felt. The research suggests how psychopaths could switch empathy on at will, which would enable them to be both callous and charming. The team who conducted the study say it is still unknown how to transform this willful empathy into the spontaneous empathy most people have, though they propose it could be possible to bring psychopaths closer to rehabilitation by helping them to activate their "empathy switch". Others suggested that despite the results of the study, it remained unclear whether psychopaths' experience of empathy was the same as that of controls, and also questioned the possibility of devising therapeutic interventions that would make the empathic reactions more automatic.[125][126]
Work conducted by Professor Jean Decety with large samples of incarcerated psychopaths offers additional insights. In one study, psychopaths were scanned while viewing video clips depicting people being intentionally hurt. They were also tested on their responses to seeing short videos of facial expressions of pain. The participants in the high-psychopathy group exhibited significantly less activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala and periaqueductal gray parts of the brain, but more activity in the striatum and the insula when compared to control participants.[127] In a second study, individuals with psychopathy exhibited a strong response in pain-affective brain regions when taking an imagine-self perspective, but failed to recruit the neural circuits that were activated in controls during an imagine-other perspective—in particular the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala—which may contribute to their lack of empathic concern.[128]
Despite studies suggesting deficits in emotion perception and imagining others in pain, professor Simon Baron-Cohen claims psychopathy is associated with intact cognitive empathy, which would imply an intact ability to read and respond to behaviors, social cues and what others are feeling. Psychopathy is, however, associated with impairment in the other major component of empathy—affective (emotional) empathy—which includes the ability to feel the suffering and emotions of others (what scientists would term as emotional contagion), and those with the condition are therefore not distressed by the suffering of their victims. Those with autism, on the other hand, are often impaired in both affective and cognitive empathy.[55]
Other conditions
Borderline personality disorder is characterized by extensive behavioral and interpersonal difficulties that arise from emotional and cognitive dysfunction.[129] Dysfunctional social and interpersonal behavior has been shown to play a crucial role in the emotionally intense way people with borderline personality disorder react.[130] While individuals with borderline personality disorder may show their emotions too much, several authors have suggested that they might have a compromised ability to reflect upon mental states (impaired cognitive empathy), as well as an impaired theory of mind.[130]
People with borderline personality disorder are very good at recognizing emotions in people's faces, suggesting increased empathic capacities.[131][132] It is, therefore, possible that impaired cognitive empathy (the capacity for understanding another person's experience and perspective) may account for borderline personality disorder individuals' tendency for interpersonal dysfunction, while "hyper-emotional empathy"[verification needed] may account for the emotional over-reactivity observed in these individuals.[130] One primary study confirmed that patients with borderline personality disorder were significantly impaired in cognitive empathy, yet there was no sign of impairment in affective empathy.[130]
One diagnostic criterion of narcissistic personality disorder is a lack of empathy and an unwillingness or inability to recognize or identify with the feelings and needs of others.[133]
Characteristics of schizoid personality disorder include emotional coldness, detachment, and impaired affect corresponding with an inability to be empathetic and sensitive towards others.[134][135][136][137]
A study conducted by Jean Decety and colleagues at the University of Chicago demonstrated that subjects with aggressive conduct disorder elicit atypical empathic responses to viewing others in pain.[89] Subjects with conduct disorder were at least as responsive as controls to the pain of others, but unlike controls, subjects with conduct disorder showed strong and specific activation of the amygdala and ventral striatum (areas that enable a general arousing effect of reward), yet impaired activation of the neural regions involved in self-regulation and metacognition (including moral reasoning), in addition to diminished processing between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.[89]
Schizophrenia is characterized by impaired affective empathy,[8][50] as well as severe cognitive and empathy impairments as measured by the Empathy Quotient (EQ).[56] These empathy impairments are also associated with impairments in social cognitive tasks.[56]
Bipolar individuals have been observed to have impaired cognitive empathy and theory of mind, but increased affective empathy.[50][138] Despite cognitive flexibility being impaired, planning behavior is intact. It has been suggested that dysfunctions in the prefrontal cortex could result in the impaired cognitive empathy, since impaired cognitive empathy has been related with neurocognitive task performance involving cognitive flexibility.[138]
Lieutenant Colonel Dave Grossman, in his book On Killing, suggests that military training artificially creates depersonalization in soldiers, suppressing empathy and making it easier for them to kill other human beings.[90]
Practical issues
This section has an unclear citation style. (January 2013) |
The capacity to empathize is a revered trait in society.[45] Empathy is considered a motivating factor for unselfish, prosocial behavior,[139] whereas a lack of empathy is related to antisocial behavior.[45][140][141]
Proper empathic engagement helps an individual understand and anticipate the behavior of another. Apart from the automatic tendency to recognize the emotions of others, one may also deliberately engage in empathic reasoning. Two general methods have been identified here.[142] An individual may simulate fictitious versions of the beliefs, desires, character traits and context of another individual to see what emotional feelings it provokes. Or, an individual may simulate an emotional feeling and then access the environment for a suitable reason for the emotional feeling to be appropriate for that specific environment.[citation needed]
Some research suggests that people are more able and willing to empathize with those most similar to themselves. In particular, empathy increases with similarities in culture and living conditions. Empathy is more likely to occur between individuals whose interaction is more frequent. (See Levenson and Reuf 1997 and Hoffman 2000: 62). A measure of how well a person can infer the specific content of another person's thoughts and feelings has been developed by William Ickes (1997, 2003). Ickes and his colleagues have developed a video-based method to measure empathic accuracy and have used this method to study the empathic inaccuracy of maritally aggressive and abusive spouses, among other topics.[citation needed]
There are concerns that the empathizer's own emotional background may affect or distort what emotions they perceive in others (e.g. Goleman 1996: p. 104). Empathy is not a process that is likely to deliver certain judgments about the emotional states of others. It is a skill that is gradually developed throughout life, and which improves the more contact we have with the person with whom one empathizes. Accordingly, any knowledge gained of the emotions of the other must be revisable in light of further information.[citation needed] Empathizers report finding it easier to take the perspective of another person when they have experienced a similar situation,[143] as well as experience greater empathic emotion.[144] Research regarding whether similar past experience makes the empathizer more accurate is mixed.[143][144]
Ethical issues
The extent to which a person's emotions are publicly observable, or mutually recognized as such has significant social consequences. Empathic recognition may or may not be welcomed or socially desirable. This is particularly the case where we recognize the emotions that someone has towards us during real time interactions. Based on a metaphorical affinity with touch, philosopher Edith Wyschogrod claims that the proximity entailed by empathy increases the potential vulnerability of either party.[145] The appropriate role of empathy in our dealings with others is highly dependent on the circumstances. For instance, Tania Singer claims that clinicians or caregivers must take care not to be too sensitive to the emotions of others, to over-invest their own emotions, at the risk of draining away their own resourcefulness.[146] Furthermore, an awareness of the limitations of empathic accuracy is prudent in a caregiving situation.
Disciplinary approaches
Philosophy
Ethics
In his 2008 book, How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time:Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, philosopher Iain King presents two reasons why empathy is the "essence" or "DNA" of right and wrong. First, he argues that empathy uniquely has all the characteristics we can know about an ethical viewpoint[147] – including that it is "partly self-standing", and so provides a source of motivation that is partly within us and partly outside, as moral motivations seem to be.[148] This allows empathy-based judgements to have sufficient distance from a personal opinion to count as "moral". His second argument is more practical: he argues, "Empathy for others really is the route to value in life", and so the means by which a selfish attitude can become a moral one.[149] By using empathy as the basis for a system of ethics, King is able to reconcile ethics based on consequences with virtue-ethics and act-based accounts of right and wrong.[150] His empathy-based system has been taken up by some Buddhists,[151] and is used to address some practical problems, such as when to tell lies,[152] and how to develop culturally-neutral rules for romance.
In the 2007 book The Ethics of Care and Empathy, philosopher Michael Slote introduces a theory of care-based ethics that is grounded in empathy. His claim is that moral motivation does, and should, stem from a basis of empathic response. He claims that our natural reaction to situations of moral significance are explained by empathy. He explains that the limits and obligations of empathy and in turn morality are natural. These natural obligations include a greater empathic, and moral obligation to family and friends, along with an account of temporal and physical distance. In situations of close temporal and physical distance, and with family or friends, our moral obligation seems stronger to us than with strangers at a distance naturally. Slote explains that this is due to empathy and our natural empathic ties. He further adds that actions are wrong if and only if they reflect or exhibit a deficiency of fully developed empathic concern for others on the part of the agent.[153]
Phenomenology
In phenomenology, empathy describes the experience of something from the other's viewpoint, without confusion between self and other. This draws on the sense of agency. In the most basic sense, this is the experience of the other's body and, in this sense, it is an experience of "my body over there". In most other respects, however, the experience is modified so that what is experienced is experienced as being the other's experience; in experiencing empathy, what is experienced is not "my" experience, even though I experience it. Empathy is also considered to be the condition of intersubjectivity and, as such, the source of the constitution of objectivity.[154]
History
Some postmodern historians such as Keith Jenkins in recent years have debated whether or not it is possible to empathize with people from the past. Jenkins argues that empathy only enjoys such a privileged position in the present because it corresponds harmoniously with the dominant liberal discourse of modern society and can be connected to John Stuart Mill's concept of reciprocal freedom. Jenkins argues the past is a foreign country and as we do not have access to the epistemological conditions of by gone ages we are unable to empathize.[155]
It is impossible to forecast the effect of empathy on the future.[citation needed] A past subject may take part in the present by the so-called historic present. If we watch from a fictitious past, can tell the present with the future tense, as it happens with the trick of the false prophecy. There is no way of telling the present with the means of the past.[156]
Evolution
An increasing number of studies in animal behavior and neuroscience claim that empathy is not restricted to humans, and is in fact as old as the mammals, or perhaps older. Examples include dolphins saving humans from drowning or from shark attacks. Professor Tom White suggests that reports of cetaceans having three times as many spindle cells — the nerve cells that convey empathy — in their brains as we do might mean these highly-social animals have a great awareness of one another's feelings.[157]
A multitude of behaviors observed in primates, both in captivity and in the wild, and in particular in bonobos, which are reported as the most empathetic of all the primates.[158][159] A recent study has demonstrated prosocial behavior elicited by empathy in rodents.[160]
Rodents have been shown to demonstrate empathy for cagemates (but not strangers) in pain.[161] One of the most widely read studies on the evolution of empathy, which discusses a neural perception-action mechanism (PAM), is the one by Stephanie Preston and de Waal.[142] This review postulates a bottom-up model of empathy that ties together all levels, from state matching to perspective-taking. For University of Chicago neurobiologist Jean Decety, [empathy] is not specific to humans. He argues that there is strong evidence that empathy has deep evolutionary, biochemical, and neurological underpinnings, and that even the most advanced forms of empathy in humans are built on more basic forms and remain connected to core mechanisms associated with affective communication, social attachment, and parental care.[11] Core neural circuits that are involved in empathy and caring include the brainstem, the amygdala, hypothalamus, basal ganglia, insula and orbitofrontal cortex.[162]
Context evolution problems
Since all definitions of empathy involves an element of for others, all distinctions between egoism and empathy fail at least for beings lacking self-awareness. Since the first mammals lacked a self-aware distinction between self and other, as shown by most mammals failing at mirror tests, the first mammals or anything more evolutionarily primitive than them cannot have had a context of default egoism requiring an empathy mechanism to be transcended. However, there are numerous examples in artificial intelligence research showing that simple reactions can carry out de facto functions the agents have no concept of, so this does not contradict evolutionary explanations of parental care. However, such mechanisms would be unadapted to self-other distinction and beings already dependent on some form of behavior benefitting each other or their offspring would never be able to evolve a form of self-other distinction that necessitated evolution of specialized non-preevolved and non-preevolvable mechanisms for retaining empathic behavior in the presence of self-other distinction, and so a fundamental neurological distinction between egoism and empathy cannot exist in any species.[163][164][165]
Psychotherapy
Heinz Kohut is the main introducer of the principle of empathy in psychoanalysis. His principle applies to the method of gathering unconscious material. The possibility of not applying the principle is granted in the cure, for instance when you must reckon with another principle, that of reality. Developing skills of empathy is often a central theme in the recovery process for drug addicts.[citation needed]
In evolutionary psychology, attempts at explaining pro-social behavior often mention the presence of empathy in the individual as a possible variable. While exact motives behind complex social behaviors are difficult to distinguish, the "ability to put oneself in the shoes of another person and experience events and emotions the way that person experienced them" is the definitive factor for truly altruistic behavior according to Batson's empathy-altruism hypothesis. If empathy is not felt, social exchange (what's in it for me?) supersedes pure altruism, but if empathy is felt, an individual will help by actions or by word, regardless of whether it is in their self-interest to do so and even if the costs outweigh potential rewards.[166]
Education
An important target of the method Learning by teaching (LbT) is to train systematically and, in each lesson, teach empathy. Students have to transmit new content to their classmates, so they have to reflect continuously on the mental processes of the other students in the classroom. This way it is possible to develop step-by-step the students' feeling for group reactions and networking. Carl R. Rogers pioneered research in effective psychotherapy and teaching which espoused that empathy coupled with unconditional positive regard or caring for students and authenticity or congruence were the most important traits for a therapist or teacher to have. Other research and publications by Tausch, Aspy, Roebuck. Lyon, and meta-analyses by Cornelius-White, corroborated the importance of these person-centered traits.[167][168]
Business and management
In the 2009 book Wired to Care, strategy consultant Dev Patnaik argues that a major flaw in contemporary business practice is a lack of empathy inside large corporations. He states that lacking any sense of empathy, people inside companies struggle to make intuitive decisions and often get fooled into believing they understand their business if they have quantitative research to rely upon. Patnaik claims that the real opportunity for companies doing business in the 21st Century is to create a widely held sense of empathy for customers, pointing to Nike, Harley-Davidson, and IBM as examples of "Open Empathy Organizations". Such institutions, he claims, see new opportunities more quickly than competitors, adapt to change more easily, and create workplaces that offer employees a greater sense of mission in their jobs.[169] In the 2011 book The Empathy Factor, organizational consultant Marie Miyashiro similarly argues the value of bringing empathy to the workplace, and offers Nonviolent Communication as an effective mechanism for achieving this.[170] In studies by the Management Research Group, empathy was found to be the strongest predictor of ethical leadership behavior out of 22 competencies in its management model, and empathy was one of the three strongest predictors of senior executive effectiveness.[171]
Intercultural
Intercultural empathy is the ability to perceive the world as it is perceived by a culture different from the subject's own. Empathy interculturally regards a variety of issues, such as the approach to time perception (deadlines, temporal precision, perspective time), how to negotiate with people from different cultures and organizations, and be able to integrate all possible difference of communication styles due to differences in culture.
Measurement
This section has an unclear citation style. (July 2013) |
Research into the measurement of empathy has sought to answer a number of questions: who should be carrying out the measurement? What should pass for empathy and what should be discounted? What unit of measure (UOM) should be adopted and to what degree should each occurrence precisely match that UOM are also key questions that researchers have sought to investigate.
Researchers have approached the measurement of empathy from a number of perspectives.
Behavioral measures normally involve raters assessing the presence or absence of certain either predetermined or ad-hoc behaviors in the subjects they are monitoring. Both verbal and non-verbal behaviors have been captured on video by experimenters such as Truax (1967b).[172] Other experimenters, including Mehrabian and Epstein (1972),[173] have required subjects to comment upon their own feelings and behaviors, or those of other people involved in the experiment, as indirect ways of signaling their level of empathic functioning to the raters.
Physiological responses tend to be captured by elaborate electronic equipment that has been physically connected to the subject's body. Researchers then draw inferences about that person's empathic reactions from the electronic readings produced (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992;[174] Leslie et al., 2004[175]).
Bodily or "somatic" measures can be looked upon as behavioral measures at a micro level. Their focus is upon measuring empathy through facial and other non-verbally expressed reactions in the empathizer. These changes are presumably underpinned by physiological changes brought about by some form of "emotional contagion" or mirroring (e.g. Levenson and Ruef, 1992*; Leslie et al., 2004*). It should be pointed out that these reactions, whilst appearing to reflect the internal emotional state of the empathizer, could also, if the stimulus incident lasted more than the briefest period, be reflecting the results of emotional reactions that are based upon more pieces of thinking through (cognitions) associated with role-taking ("if I were him I would feel ...").
Paper-based indices involve one or more of a variety of methods of responding. In some experiments, subjects are required to watch video scenarios (either staged or authentic) and to make written responses which are then assessed for their levels of empathy (e.g. Geher, Warner and Brown, 2001[176]); scenarios are sometimes also depicted in printed form (e.g. Mehrabian and Epstein, 1972[173]). Measures also frequently require subjects to self-report upon their own ability or capacity for empathy, using Likert-style numerical responses to a printed questionnaire that may have been designed to tap into the affective, cognitive-affective or largely cognitive substrates of empathic functioning. Some questionnaires claim to have been able to tap into both cognitive and affective substrates (e.g. Davis, 1980[177]). More recent paper-based tools include The Empathy Quotient (EQ) created by Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright[178] which comprises a self-report questionnaire consisting of 60 items.
For the very young, picture or puppet-story indices for empathy have been adopted to enable even very young, pre-school subjects to respond without needing to read questions and write answers (e.g. Denham and Couchoud, 1990). Dependent variables (variables that are monitored for any change by the experimenter) for younger subjects have included self reporting on a 7-point smiley face scale and filmed facial reactions (Barnett, 1984).[179]
A certain amount of confusion exists about how to measure empathy. These may be rooted in another problem: deciding what empathy is and what it is not. In general, researchers have until now been keen to pin down a singular definition of empathy which would allow them to design a measure to assess its presence in an exchange, in someone's repertoire of behaviors or within them as a latent trait. As a result, they have been frequently forced to ignore the richness of the empathic process in favor of capturing surface, explicit self-report or third-party data about whether empathy between two people was present or not. In most cases, instruments have unfortunately only yielded information on whether someone had the potential to demonstrate empathy (Geher et al., 2001)*. Gladstein (1987)[180] summarizes the position noting that empathy has been measured from the point of view of the empathizer, the recipient for empathy and the third-party observer. He suggests that since the multiple measures used have produced results that bear little relation to one another, researchers should refrain from making comparisons between scales that are in fact measuring different things. He suggests that researchers should instead stipulate what kind of empathy they are setting out to measure rather than simplistically stating that they are setting out to measure the unitary phenomenon "empathy"; a view more recently endorsed by Duan and Hill (1996).[181]
In the field of medicine, a measurement tool for carers is the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, Health Professional Version (JSPE-HP).[182] At least one study using this tool with health sciences' students has found that levels of empathy are greater amongst females than males, and also are greater amongst older students than younger students.[183]
The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is the only published measurement tool accounting for a multi-dimensional assessment of empathy, consisting of a self-report questionnaire of 28 items, divided into four 7-item scales covering the subdivisions of affective and cognitive empathy.[45][51]
Other animals
Research has shown that the ability of empathy in other species is attainable. Many instances of empathy have been recorded throughout many species, including (but not limited to) canines, felines, dolphins, primates, rats and mice [citation needed]. In animals, empathy-related responding could have an ulterior motive such as survival, the sharing of food, companionship and pack-oriented mentality. It is certainly difficult to understand an animal's intention behind an empathic response. Many researchers[who?] maintain that applying the term empathy in general to animal behavior is an act of anthropomorphism.
Researchers Zanna Clay and Frans de Waal studied the socio-emotional development of the bonobo chimpanzee.[184] They focused on the interplay of numerous skills such as empathy-related responding, and how different rearing backgrounds of the juvenile bonobo affected their response to stressful events, related to themselves (loss of a fight) and of stressful events of others. It was found that the bonobos sought out body contact as a coping mechanism with one another. A finding of this study was that the bonobos sought out more body contact after watching a distressing event upon the other bonobos rather than their individually experienced stressful event. Mother-reared bonobos as opposed to orphaned bonobos sought out more physical contact after a stressful event happened to another. This finding shows the importance of mother-child attachment and bonding, and how it may be crucial to successful socio-emotional development, such as empathic-like behaviors.
Empathic-like responding has been observed in chimpanzees in various different aspects of their natural behaviors. For example, chimpanzees are known to spontaneously contribute comforting behaviors to victims of aggressive behavior in natural and unnatural settings, a behavior recognized as consolation. Researchers Teresa Romero and co-workers observed these empathic and sympathetic-like behaviors in chimpanzees at two separate outdoor housed groups.[185] The act of consolation was observed in both of the groups of chimpanzees. This behavior is found in humans, and particularly in human infants. Another similarity found between chimpanzees and humans is that empathic-like responding was disproportionately provided to individuals of kin. Although comforting towards non-family chimpanzees was also observed, as with humans, chimpanzees showed the majority of comfort and concern to close/loved ones. Another similarity between chimpanzee and human expression of empathy is that females provided more comfort than males on average. The only exception to this discovery was that high-ranking males showed as much empathy-like behavior as their female counterparts. This is believed to be because of policing-like behavior and the authoritative status of high-ranking male chimpanzees.
It is thought that species that possess a more intricate and developed prefrontal cortex have more of an ability of experiencing empathy. It has however been found that empathic and altruistic responses may also be found in sand dwelling Mediterranean ants. Researcher Hollis studied the Cataglyphis cursor sand dwelling Mediterranean ant and their rescue behaviors by ensnaring ants from a nest in nylon threads and partially buried beneath the sand.[186] The ants not ensnared in the nylon thread proceeded to attempt to rescue their nest mates by sand digging, limb pulling, transporting sand away from the trapped ant, and when efforts remained unfruitful, began to attack the nylon thread itself; biting and pulling apart the threads. Similar rescue behavior was found in other sand-dwelling Mediterranean ants, but only Cataglyphis floricola and Lasius grandis species of ants showed the same rescue behaviors of transporting sand away from the trapped victim and directing attention towards the nylon thread. It was observed in all ant species that rescue behavior was only directed towards nest mates. Ants of the same species from different nests were treated with aggression and were continually attacked and pursued, which speaks to the depths of ants discriminative abilities. This study brings up the possibility that if ants have the capacity for empathy and/or altruism, these complex processes may be derived from primitive and simpler mechanisms.
Canines have been hypothesized to share empathic-like responding towards human species. Researchers Custance and Mayer put individual dogs in an enclosure with their owner and a stranger.[187] When the participants were talking or humming, the dog showed no behavioral changes, however when the participants were pretending to cry, the dogs oriented their behavior toward the person in distress whether it be the owner or stranger. The dogs approached the participants when crying in a submissive fashion, by sniffing, licking and nuzzling the distressed person. The dogs did not approach the participants in the usual form of excitement, tail wagging or panting. Since the dogs did not direct their empathic-like responses only towards their owner, it is hypothesized that dogs generally seek out humans showing distressing body behavior. Although this could insinuate that dogs have the cognitive capacity for empathy, this could also mean that domesticated dogs have learned to comfort distressed humans through generations of being rewarded for that specific behavior.
When witnessing chicks in distress, domesticated hens, Gallus gallus domesticus show emotional and physiological responding. Researchers Edgar, Paul and Nicol [188] found that in conditions where the chick was susceptible to danger, the mother hens heart rate increased, vocal alarms were sounded, personal preening decreased and body temperature increased. This responding happened whether or not the chick felt as if they were in danger. Mother hens experienced stress-induced hyperthermia only when the chick's behavior correlated with the perceived threat. Animal maternal behavior may be perceived as empathy, however, it could be guided by the evolutionary principles of survival and not emotionality.
See also
- Attribution (psychology)
- Emotional contagion
- Emotional intelligence
- Emotional literacy
- Empathic concern
- Empathizing–systemizing theory
- Ethnocultural empathy
- Grounding in communication
- Highly sensitive person
- Humanistic coefficient
- Identification (psychology)
- Life skills
- Mimpathy
- Oxytocin
- People skills
- Philip K. Dick's Do Androids Dream of Electric Sheep?
- Schema (psychology)
- Self-conscious emotions
- Simulation theory of empathy
- Social emotions
- Soft skills
- Theory of mind in animals
References
- ^ Bellet, Paul S., and Michael J. Maloney (1991). "The importance of empathy as an interviewing skill in medicine". JAMA. 226 (13): 1831–1832. doi:10.1001/jama.1991.03470130111039.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Rothschild, B. (with Rand, M. L.). (2006). Help for the Helper: The psychophysiology of compassion fatigue and vicarious trauma. New York, NY: Norton.
- ^ Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist, 17(1), 18-24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268
- ^ Empatheia, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, at Perseus
- ^ Titchener E.B. (1909/2014) Introspection and empathy Dialogues in Philosophy, Mental and Neuro Sciences 2014; 7: 25–30
- ^ Gallese, Vittorio (2003). "The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity". Psychopathology. 36 (4): 171–180. doi:10.1159/000072786. PMID 14504450. CiteSeerx: 10.1.1.143.2396.
- ^ Bar-On, Reuven; Parker, James DA (2000). The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace. San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 0-7879-4984-1.
- ^ a b c G H M Pijnenborg, G.H.M., Spikman, J.M., Jeronimus, B.F., Aleman, A. (2012). "Insight in schizophrenia: associations with empathy". European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 263 (4): 299–307. doi:10.1007/s00406-012-0373-0. PMID 23076736.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Hodges, S.D., & Klein, K.J. (2001). Regulating the costs of empathy: the price of being human. Journal of Socio-Economics.
- ^ a b Snyder, C. R., Shane J. Lopez, and Jennifer T. Pedrotti. Positive Psychology: The Scientific and Practical Explorations of Human Strengths. Second ed. Los Angeles: SAGE, 2011. 267–75. Print.
- ^ a b Decety J (2011). "The neuroevolution of empathy". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1231 (1): 35–45. Bibcode:2011NYASA1231...35D. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x. PMID 21651564.
- ^ "Empathy". plato.stanford.edu. March 31, 2008. Retrieved August 29, 2012.
- ^ Happiness Genes: Unlock the Positive Potential Hidden in Your DNA, New Page Books (April, 2010) ISBN 978-1-60163-105-3
- ^ O'Malley W. J. (1999). "Teaching Empathy". America. 180 (12): 22–26.
- ^ Schwartz W (2002). "From passivity to competence: A conceptualization of knowledge, skill, tolerance, and empathy". Psychiatry. 65 (4): 338–345. doi:10.1521/psyc.65.4.338.20239.
- ^ Schwartz W (2013). "The parameters of empathy: Core considerations for psychotherapy and supervision". Advances in Descriptive Psychology. 10. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2393689.
- ^ Meltzoff A.N., Decety J. (2003). "What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 358 (1431): 491–500. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1261.
- ^ Batson, C.D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In J. Decety and W. Ickes (Eds.), The Social Neuroscience of Empathy (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: MIT Press
- ^ Hatfield E., Cacioppo J. L., Rapson R. L. (1993). "Emotional contagion" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Sciences. 2 (3): 96–99. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770953. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 19, 2012.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c The Essential Difference: The Truth about the Male and Female Brain, Basic Books (July 1, 2003) ISBN 978-0-7382-0844-2
- ^ Cheng Y., Tzeng O.J., Decety J., Hsieh J.C. (2006). "Gender differences in the human mirror system: a magnetoencephalography study". NeuroReport. 17 (11): 1115–1119. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21. PMID 16837838.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Cheng Y., Decety J., Hsieh J.C., Hung D., Tzeng O.J. (2007). "Gender differences in spinal excitability during observation of bipedal locomotion". NeuroReport. 18 (9): 887–890. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280ebb486. PMID 17515795.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Yang C.Y., Decety J., Lee S., Chen G., Cheng Y. (2009). "Gender differences in the Mu rhythm during empathy for pain: An electroencephalographic study". Brain Research. 1251: 176–184. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.062. PMID 19083993.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Cheng Y., Lee P., Yang C.Y., Lin C.P., Decety J.; Lee; Yang; Lin; Hung; Decety (2008). Rustichini, Aldo (ed.). "Gender differences in the mu rhythm of the human mirror-neuron system". PLoS ONE. 5 (5): e2113. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2113C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002113. PMC 2361218. PMID 18461176.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Proverbio, Alice Mado; Riva, Federica; Zani, Alberto. "When neurons do not mirror the agent's intentions: Sex differences in neural coding of goal-directed actions". Neuropsychologia. 48 (5): 1454–1463. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.015.
- ^ Ickes, W. (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press.
- ^ Klein K. Hodges S. (2001). "Gender Differences, Motivation, and Empathic Accuracy: When it Pays to Understand". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 27 (6): 720–730. doi:10.1177/0146167201276007.
- ^ a b Kret, M. E.; De Gelder, B. (June 1, 2012). "A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals". Neuropsychologia. 50 (7): 1211–1221. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022.
- ^ "Meta-analysis reveals adult female superiority in "Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test"". ResearchGate. Retrieved December 1, 2015.
- ^ Thompson, Ashley E.; Voyer, Daniel (January 1, 2014). "Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis". Cognition & Emotion. 28 (7): 1164–1195. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875889. ISSN 1464-0600. PMID 24400860.
- ^ Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ, Frith CD; Seymour; O'Doherty; Stephan; Dolan; Frith (January 2006). "Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others". Nature. 439 (7075): 466–9. Bibcode:2006Natur.439..466S. doi:10.1038/nature04271. PMC 2636868. PMID 16421576.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f g h Christov-Moore, Leonardo; Simpson, Elizabeth A.; Coudé, Gino; Grigaityte, Kristina; Iacoboni, Marco; Ferrari, Pier Francesco. "Empathy: Gender effects in brain and behavior". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 46: 604–627. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001.
- ^ Marjanovic, Zdravko; Struthers, Greenglass (August 8, 2011). "Who Helps Natural-Disaster Victims? Assessment of Trait and Situational Predictors" (PDF). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 12 (1): 245–267. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2011.01262.x.
- ^ Einolf, Christopher (March 13, 2012). "Is Cognitive Empathy More Important than Affective Empathy? A Response to "Who Helps Natural-Disaster Victims?"" (PDF). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 12 (1): 268–271. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2012.01281.x. Retrieved May 30, 2014.
- ^ Dovidio J.F., Allen J.L., Schroeder D.A. (1990). "Specificity of empathy-induced helping: Evidence for altruistic motivation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59 (2): 249–260. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.2.249.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Keen Suzanne (2006). "A Theory of Narrative Empathy". Narrative. 14 (3): 207–36. doi:10.1353/nar.2006.0015.
- ^ Bartlett, M. Y., and D. Desteno. (2006) "Gratitude and Prosocial Behavior: Helping When It Costs You." Psychological Science 17.4 : 319–25.
- ^ Bartlett M. Y., Desteno D. (2006). "Gratitude and Prosocial Behavior: Helping When It Costs You". Psychological Science. 17 (4): 319–325. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x.
- ^ McAlinden,M. (2014) Can teachers know learners' minds? Teacher empathy and learner body language in English language teaching. In Critical Perspectives in Language Education. Springer.
- ^ a b Tettegah S., Anderson C. J. (2007). "Pre-service teachers' empathy and cognitions: Statistical analysis of text data by graphical models". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 32 (1): 48–82. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.010.
- ^ Keillor, Robin Michelle. "Empathy and Intergroup Relations: A Study in Cross-Cultural Relationship Building." Order No. AEH9940789 2000. ProQuest. Web. 13 Apr. 2014.
- ^ Tisot, Caroline Teresa Monforte. "Environmental Contributions to Empathy Development in Young Children." Order No. AAI3097734 2004. ProQuest. Web. 13 Apr. 2014.
- ^ Batson C. D., Moran T. (1999). "Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner's dilemma". Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29 (7): 909–924. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0992(199911)29:7<909::aid-ejsp965>3.0.co;2-l.
- ^ Snyder, C.R. and Lopez, S.J. (Eds.). (2009). Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology. Second ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 243–44.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (April 2007). "Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 37 (4): 709–15. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f g Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Judith Aharon-Peretz and Daniella Perry (2009). "Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions". Brain. 132 (3): 617–627. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279.
- ^ Frans B.M. deWaal (2008). "Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy" (PDF). Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59 (1): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PMID 17550343.
- ^ Gerace, A.; Day, A.; Casey, S.; Mohr, P. (2013). "An exploratory investigation of the process of perspective taking in interpersonal situations". Journal of Relationships Research. 4: e6, 1–12. doi:10.1017/jrr.2013.6.
{{cite journal}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - ^ Kimberley Rogers, Isabel Dziobek, Jason Hassenstab, Oliver T. Wolf, Antonio Convit (2007). "Who Cares? Revisiting Empathy in Asperger Syndrome" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 37 (4): 709–715. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d e f g Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. (August 2012). "The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics". Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 7 (6): 727–37. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr051. PMC 3427869. PMID 21896497.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c d Davis M (1983). "Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 44 (1): 113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113.
- ^ a b c d e f g h i Ilaria Minio-Paluello, Michael V. Lombardo, Bhismadev Chakrabarti, Sally Wheelwright, Simon Baron-Cohen (2009). "Response to Smith's Letter to the Editor 'Emotional Empathy in Autism Spectrum Conditions: Weak, Intact, or Heightened?'" (PDF). J Autism Dev Disord. 39 (12): 1749–1754. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0800-x.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural basis of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Phoebe Caldwell, "Letters", London Times, Dec 30 2005
- ^ a b c Baron-Cohen, Simon (2011). Zero Degrees of Empathy: A New Theory of Human Cruelty. Penguin UK. ISBN 9780713997910. Retrieved August 8, 2013.
- ^ a b c Bora, E.; Gökçen, S.; Veznedaroglu, B. (2008). "Empathic abilities in people with schizophrenia". Psychiatry Res. 160 (1): 23–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.017. PMID 18514324.
- ^ Yan Fan, Niall W. Duncana, Moritz de Greck, Georg Northoff (2011). "Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 35 (3): 903–911. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Hoffman, M.L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
- ^ Decety J., Meyer M. (2008). "From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: A social developmental neuroscience account". Development and Psychopathology. 20 (4): 1053–1080. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000503. PMID 18838031.
- ^ Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T.L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006) Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of Moral Development (pp. 517–549). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
- ^ Falck-Ytter, T., Gredebäck, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people's action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9
- ^ Zahn-Waxler C., Radke-Yarrow M. (1990). "The origins of empathic concern". Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 107–130. doi:10.1007/BF00991639.
- ^ Decety J., Michalska K.J., Akitsuki Y. (2008). "Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children". Neuropsychologia. 46 (11): 2607–2614. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.026. PMID 18573266.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Brain Scans Show Children Naturally Prone to Empathy Newswise. Retrieved July 13, 2008.
- ^ Wimmer H., Perner J. (1983). "Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception". Cognition. 13 (1): 103–28. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. PMID 6681741.
- ^ Olsen, Douglas (September 2001). "Empathetic Maturity: Theory of Moral Point of View in Clinical Relations". Advances in Nursing Science. 24 (1): 36–46. doi:10.1097/00012272-200109000-00006. PMID 11554532.
- ^ Davis, Mark H. "Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach." Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44.1(1983): 113–26. Print.
- ^ Haas, BW; Brook, M; Remillard, L; Ishak, A; Anderson, IW; Filkowski, MM (2015). "I know how you feel: the warm-altruistic personality profile and the empathic brain". PLOS ONE. 10 (3): e0120639. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120639. PMID 25769028.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Keysers, C. & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions and sensations. Curr Opinion Neurobiol, 2009
- ^ Decety J., Moriguchi Y. (2007). "The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions". BioPsychoSocial Medicine. 1 (1): 22–65. doi:10.1186/1751-0759-1-22.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Wicker B.; Keysers, Christian; Plailly, Jane; Royet, Jean-Pierre; Gallese, Vittorio; Rizzolatti, Giacomo; et al. (2003). "Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust". Neuron. 40 (3): 655–664. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287.
- ^ Keysers, C. et al. (2004). A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch, Neuron, 42:335–46
- ^ Blakemore S.-J.; Bristow, D; Bird, G; Frith, C; Ward, J; et al. (2005). "Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia". Brain. 128 (Pt 7): 1571–1583. doi:10.1093/brain/awh500. PMID 15817510.
- ^ Morrison, I.; Lloyd, D.; di Pellegrino, G.; Roberts, N. (2004). "Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue?". Cognitive & Affective Behavioral Neuroscience. 4 (2): 270–278. doi:10.3758/cabn.4.2.270.
- ^ Jackson P.L., Meltzoff A.N., Decety J. (2005). "How do we perceive the pain of others: A window into the neural processes involved in empathy". NeuroImage. 24 (3): 771–779. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006. PMID 15652312.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Lamm C., Batson C.D., Decety J. (2007). "The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Singer T.; Seymour; O'Doherty; Kaube; Dolan; Frith; et al. (2004). "Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain". Science. 303 (5661): 1157–1161. Bibcode:2004Sci...303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535.
- ^ Preston S., de Waal F. (2002). "Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25 (1): 1–72. doi:10.1017/s0140525x02000018.
- ^ Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during obser- vation of outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (2010), doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011
- ^ Jack, Anthony I.; Dawson, Abigail J.; Begany, Katelyn L.; Leckie, Regina L.; Barry, Kevin P.; Ciccia, Angela H.; Snyder, Abraham Z. (2013). "FMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains". NeuroImage. 66: 385–401. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.061.
- ^ "Empathy represses analytic thought, and vice versa: Brain physiology limits simultaneous use of both networks". ScienceDaily.
- ^ Eres, Robert; Decety, Jean; Louis, Winnifred R.; Molenberghs, Pascal. "Individual differences in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and cognitive empathy". NeuroImage. 117: 305–310. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.038.
- ^ Thomas, Ben (November 6, 2012). What’s so special about mirror neurons? New York: Scientific American Guest Blog.
- ^ March, J. (March 29, 2012). Do mirror neurons give us empathy? Berkeley: Greater Good Science Center.
- ^ a b Hoffman, Martin L. (1990). "Empathy and justice motivation". Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 151–172. doi:10.1007/BF00991641. Retrieved August 29, 2012.
- ^ Abstract: Assessing a new dimension of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and punishing desires, Vitaglione, Guy D. & Barnett, Mark A. [1]
- ^ Mohr P., Howells K., Gerace A., Day A., Wharton M. (2007). "The role of perspective taking in anger arousal". Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (3): 507–517. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.019.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Day A., Mohr P., Howells K., Gerace A., Lim L. (2012). "The role of empathy in anger arousal in violent offenders and university students". International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 56 (4): 599–613. doi:10.1177/0306624X11431061.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b c Decety, J., Michalska, K.J., Akitsuki, Y., & Lahey, B. (2008). "Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation". Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–11. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC 2819310. PMID 18940230.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b Grossman, Dave (1996). On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society. Back Bay Books. ISBN 0-316-33000-0.
- ^ a b Hill E., Berthoz S., Frith U (2004). "'Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives.'" (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 229–235. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022613.41399.14. PMID 15162941. Archived from the original (PDF) on June 19, 2013.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Taylor, G.J. and Bagby, R.M & Parker, J.D.A. Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. (1997) Cambridge Uni. Press.
- ^ Sifneos PE. The prevalence of 'alexithymic' characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and psychosomatics, 22 (2):255–262, 1973
- ^ Moriguchi, Y., Decety, J., Ohnishi, T., Maeda, M., Matsuda, H., & Komaki, G. Empathy and judging other's pain: An fMRI study of alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex (2007); Bird, J., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U., and Frith, C. Alexithymia In Autistic Spectrum Disorders: and fMRI Investigation (2006) : and Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, U & C. Alexithymia in Autism Spectrum Disorders: an fMRI Investigation (2006).
- ^ Brackett; Warner, Rebecca M.; Bosco, Jennifer S.; et al. (2005). "'Emotional Intelligence and Relationship Quality Among Couples'" (PDF). Personal Relationships. 12 (2): 197–212. doi:10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00111.x. Archived from the original (PDF) on September 27, 2007.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help) - ^ Yelsma, P., Marrow, S. – 'An Examination of Couples' Difficulties With Emotional Expressiveness and Their Marital Satisfaction' in Journal of Family Communication 3 (2003) p. 41–62 [2]
- ^ Allen. – 'Autism, music, and the therapeutic potential of music in alexithymia' in Music Perception (2010) p.251
- ^ "Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism".
- ^ Dapretto M., Davies M.S., Pfeifer J.H., Scott A.A., Sigman M., Bookheimer S.Y., Iacoboni M. (2006). "Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders". Nature Neuroscience. 9 (1): 28–31. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMC 3713227. PMID 16327784.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Oberman. – 'EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum' in Brain Research (2005) p.190
- ^ Gillberg, C. L. (1992). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1991. Autism and autistic-like conditions: subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 33(5), 813–842.
- ^ Roeyers, H., Buysse, A., Ponnet, K., & Pichal, B. (2001). Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 271–278.
- ^ a b c Simon Baron-Cohen (2009). "Autism: The Empathizing–Systemizing (E-S) Theory". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156 (The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009). New York Academy of Sciences: 68–80. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156...68B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x. PMID 19338503. Retrieved July 29, 2013.
- ^ Baron-Cohen. 'Sex Differences in the Brain: Implications for Explaining Autism' in Science (2005) p.819
- ^ Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., et al. (2009). Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Br. J. Psychol., 100, 1–22.
- ^ "Testosterone may reduce empathy by reducing brain connectivity". PsyPost. Retrieved April 3, 2016.
- ^ "Autism 'affects male and female brains differently'". BBC News. August 9, 2013. Retrieved August 9, 2013.
- ^ Cleckly, H. C. (1941). "The Mask of Sanity: An attempt to Reinterpret the So-Called Psychopathic Personality". St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Hare, R. D. (1991). "The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised". Toronto: Multi Health Systems.
{{cite journal}}
: Cite journal requires|journal=
(help) - ^ Skeem, J. L.; Polaschek, D. L. L.; Patrick, C. J.; Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). "Psychopathic Personality: Bridging the Gap Between Scientific Evidence and Public Policy". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 12 (3): 95–162. doi:10.1177/1529100611426706.
- ^ Patrick, Christopher (2005). Handbook of Psychopathy. Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-60623-804-2.[page needed]
- ^ Andrade, Joel (March 23, 2009). Handbook of Violence Risk Assessment and Treatment: New Approaches for Mental Health Professionals. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company. ISBN 978-0-8261-9904-1. Retrieved January 5, 2014.
- ^ WHO (2010) ICD-10: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines: Disorders of adult personality and behavior
- ^ Decety, J., & Skelly, L. (2013). The neural underpinnings of the experience of empathy: Lessons for psychopathy. In K. N. Ochsner and S. M. Kosslyn (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience – Volume 2 (pp. 228–243). New York: Oxford University Press.
- ^ Kiehl K. A. (2006). "A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction". Psychiatry Research. 142 (2–3): 107–128. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013. PMC 2765815. PMID 16712954.
- ^ Blair, R.J. (1995). "A cognitive developmental approach to morality: investigating the psychopath" (PDF). Cognition. 57: 1–29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-p. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 21, 2013.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help) - ^ Blair, R.J.R. (2003). "Neurobiological basis of psychopathy". British Journal of Psychiatry. 182: 5–7. doi:10.1192/bjp.182.1.5. PMID 12509310.
- ^ a b "Psychopathy" by Quinton 2006
- ^ a b Blair, R.J.; E. Colledge, D.G. Mitchell (2001a). "Somatic markers and response reversal: is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies?". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology.
- ^ a b Blair, R. J.; D.G. Mitchell, R.A. Richell; et al. (2002). "Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 111: 682–686. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.111.4.682.
- ^ a b Stevens, D.; T. Charman, R.J. Blair (2001). "Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies". Journal of Genetic Psychology.
- ^ Decety L., Skelly L. R., Yoder K. J., Kiehl K.; Skelly; Yoder; Kiehl (2014). "Neural processing of dynamic facial expressions in psychopaths". Social Neuroscience. 9 (1): 36–49. doi:10.1080/17470919.2013.866905. PMC 3970241. PMID 24359488.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Dawel A., O'Kearney R., McKone E., Palermo R.; o’Kearney; McKone; Palermo (2012). "Not just fear and sadness: Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 36 (10): 2288–2304. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006. PMID 22944264.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Hogenboom, Melissa (July 25, 2013). "Psychopathic criminals have empathy switch". BBC News. Retrieved July 28, 2013.
- ^ Lewis, Tanya. "Cold-hearted Psychopaths Feel Empathy Too". Live Science. 24 July 2013.
- ^ Decety, J.; Skelly, L. R.; Kiehl, K. A. (2013). "Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy". JAMA Psychiatry. 70 (6): 638–645. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27.
- ^ Decety, J.; Chen, C.; Harenski, C. L.; Kiehl, K. A. (2013). "An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 489. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00489.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: unflagged free DOI (link) - ^ Minzenberg, M.J.; Fisher-Irving, M.; Poole, J.H.; Vinogradov, S. (2006). "Reduced self-referential source memory performance is associated with interpersonal dysfunction in borderline personality disorder" (PDF). Journal of Personality Disorders.
- ^ a b c d Harari, Hagai; Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory; Milli Ravid; Yechiel Levkovitz (March 1, 2009). "Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder". Psychiatry Research. 175 (3): 277–279. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002.
- ^ Wagner, A.W.; M.M Linehan (1999). "Facial expression recognition ability among women with borderline personality disorder: implications for emotion regulation?". Journal of Personality Disorders.
- ^ Lynch, T.R.; Rosenthal, M.Z.; Kosson, D.S.; Cheavens, J.S.; Lejuez, C.W.; Blair, R.J. (2006). "Heightened sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline personality disorder". Emotion. 6 (4): 647–655. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.4.647.
- ^ Narcissistic personality disorder – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
- ^ Schizoid personality disorder, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
- ^ "Archived copy". Archived from the original on April 25, 2006. Retrieved December 13, 2012.
{{cite web}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help)CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link) Schizoid personality disorder – International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision (ICD-10)] - ^ Guntrip, Harry. Schizoid Phenomena, Object-Relations, and The Self. New York: International Universities Press, 1969.
- ^ Ralph Klein- pp. 13–23 in Disorders of the Self: New Therapeutic Horizons, Brunner/Mazel (1995).
- ^ a b S., Shamay-Tsoory; H., Harari; O., Szepsenwol; Y., Levkovitz (2009). "Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder". J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 21 (1): 59–67. doi:10.1176/appi.neuropsych.21.1.59. PMID 19359453.
{{cite journal}}
:|access-date=
requires|url=
(help) - ^ Eisenberg N., Miller P. A. (1987). "The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors". Psychological Bulletin. 101 (1): 91–119. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91.
- ^ Bjorkqvist K., Osterman K., Kaukiainen A. (2000). "Social intelligence-empathy = aggression?". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 5 (2): 191–200. doi:10.1016/s1359-1789(98)00029-9.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Geer J. H., Estupinan L. A., Manguno-Mire G. M. (2000). "Empathy, social skills, and other relevant cognitive processes in rapists and child molesters". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 5 (1): 99–126. doi:10.1016/s1359-1789(98)00011-1.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ a b de Waal F. B. M. (2008). "Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy". Annual Review of Psychology. 59 (1): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PMID 17550343.
- ^ a b Gerace, A.; Day, A.; Casey, S.; Mohr, P. (2015). "Perspective taking and empathy: Does having similar past experience to another person make it easier to take their perspective?". Journal of Relationships Research. 6: e10, 1–14. doi:10.1017/jrr.2015.6.
- ^ a b Hodges, S. D.; Kiel, K. J.; Kramer, A. D. I.; Veach, D.; Villanueva, B. R. "Giving birth to empathy: The effects of similar experience on empathic accuracy, empathic concern, and perceived empathy". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (3): 398–409. doi:10.1177/0146167209350326.
- ^ Wyschogrod E (1981). "Empathy and sympathy as tactile encounter". J Med Philos. 6 (1): 25–43. doi:10.1093/jmp/6.1.25. PMID 7229562.
- ^ "Compassion over empathy could help prevent emotional burnout". Wired UK.
- ^ How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time: Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, (2008), ISBN 978-1-84706-347-2
- ^ Iain King (October 16, 2008). How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time. Continuum. p. 74. ISBN 978-1-84706-347-2. Retrieved August 28, 2013.
Empathy is special, because it always and automatically has the characteristics of right and wrong ... Something rooted in empathy must have more of the essence of good about it than something which is not.
- ^ Iain King (October 16, 2008). How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time. Continuum. p. 227. ISBN 978-1-84706-347-2. Retrieved August 28, 2013.
- ^ Peter Vardy and Charlotte Vardy (April 2012). Ethics Matters. SCM Press. p. 256. ISBN 978-0-334-04391-1. Retrieved August 28, 2013. Page 116 of this book states: In How to Make Good Decisions and Be Right All the Time: Solving the Riddle of Right and Wrong, London: Continuum 2008, Iain King develops a system compatible with consequence-, virtue- and act based ethics.
- ^ A Buddhist account of Iain King's ideas is set out in this Archived 2012-10-20 at the Wayback Machine Global Oneness article.
- ^ Publishers Weekly state that "King is even able to formulate a credible rule that tells us when to lie" here.
- ^ The Ethics of Care and Empathy, Michael Slote, Oxford University Press, 2007
- ^ Empathy in the Context of Philosophy, Lou Agosta, Palgrave/Macmillan, 2010
- ^ Jenkins, K. (1991) Re-thinking History London: Routledge
- ^ Pozzi, G. (1976) Prefazione 6. L'elemento storico e politico -sociale, in G.B. Marino, L'Adone Milano
- ^ White, T. I. (2007). In defense of dolphins: the new moral frontier. Malden, MA: Blackwell Pub..
- ^ Sandin, Jo (2007). Bonobos: Encounters in Empathy. Milwaukee: Zoological Society of Milwaukee & The Foundation for Wildlife Conservation, Inc. p. 109. ISBN 978-0-9794151-0-4.
- ^ The age of empathy: nature's lessons for a kinder society By: Waal, F. B. M. de. Harmony Books 2009
- ^ Ben-Ami Bartal I., Decety J., Mason P.; Decety; Mason (2011). "Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats". Science. 334 (6061): 1427–1430. Bibcode:2011Sci...334.1427B. doi:10.1126/science.1210789. PMC 3760221. PMID 22158823.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Dale J. Langford, Sara E. Crager, Zarrar Shehzad, Shad B. Smith, Susana G. Sotocinal, Jeremy S. Levenstadt, Mona Lisa Chanda, Daniel J. Levitin, Jeffrey S. Mogil (June 30, 2006). "Social Modulation of Pain as Evidence for Empathy in Mice". Science. 312 (5782): 1967–1970. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1967L. doi:10.1126/science.1128322. PMID 16809545.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Decety J., Svetlova M. (2012). "Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy". Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2 (1): 1–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003. PMID 22682726.
- ^ How the Body Shapes the Way We Think: A New View of Intelligence, 2006, by Rolf Pfeifer, Josh Bongard
- ^ Intelligence of Apes and Other Rational Beings 2003, Duane M. Rumbaugh and David A. Washburn
- ^ The Unpredictable Species: What Makes Humans Unique, 2013, Philip Lieberman
- ^ Aronson, Elliot; Wilson, Timothy D.; Akert, Robin (2007). Social Psychology, 6th Edition. Prentice Hall. ISBN 0-13-238245-8.
- ^ Trevisani, Daniele (2005) Negoziazione Interculturale. Comunicazione oltre le barriere culturali, Milan: Franco Angeli (Title translation: Intercultural Negotiation: Communication Beyond Cultural Barriers)
- ^ Cornelius-White,J. H. D., and A. P. Harbaugh. (2010). Learner-Centered Instruction.Thousand Oaks, CA,London, New Delhi, Singapore: SAGE Publications.
- ^ "Wired To Care". wiredtocare.com.
- ^ Miyashiro, Marie R. (2011). The Empathy Factor: Your Competitive Advantage for Personal, Team, and Business Success. Puddledancer Press. p. 256. ISBN 1-892005-25-5.
- ^ Dowden, Craig (June 21, 2013). "Forget ethics training: Focus on empathy". The National Post. Archived from the original on July 24, 2013.
- ^ Truax, C. B. (1967). Rating of Accurate Empathy. The Therapeutic Relationship and its Impact. A Study of Psychotherapy with Schizophrenics. Eds. C. R. Rogers, E. T. Gendlin, D. J. Kiesler and C. B. Truax. Madison, Wisconsin, The University of Wisconsin Press pp. 555–568.
- ^ a b Mehrabian A., Epstein N. (1972). "A measure of emotional empathy". Journal of Personality. 40 (4): 525–543. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00078.x. PMID 4642390.
- ^ Levenson R. W., Ruef A. M. (1992). "Empathy: a physiological substrate". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63 (2): 234–246. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234. PMID 1403614.
- ^ Leslie K. R., Johnson-Frey S. H.; et al. (2004). "Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy". NeuroImage. 21 (2): 601–607. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.038. PMID 14980562.
- ^ Geher G., Warner R. M.; et al. (2001). "Predictive validity of the emotional accuracy research scale". Intelligence. 29 (5): 373–388. doi:10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00045-3.
- ^ Davis M. H. (1980). "A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy". JSAS Catalogue of selected documents in psychology. 10 (4): 1–17.
- ^ Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S. (2004). "The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences" (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 163–175. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00. PMID 15162935. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 4, 2015.
{{cite journal}}
: Unknown parameter|deadurl=
ignored (|url-status=
suggested) (help) - ^ Barnett M. A. (1984). "Similarity of experience and empathy in preschoolers". Journal of Genetic Psychology. 145 (2): 241–250. doi:10.1080/00221325.1984.10532271.
- ^ Gladstein, G. A. (1987). What It All Means. Empathy and counseling: explorations in theory and research. G. A. Gladstein. New York, Springer-Verlag: 173–189.
- ^ Duan C., Hill C. E. (1996). "The current state of empathy research". Journal of Counselling Psychology. 43 (3): 261–274. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.43.3.261.
- ^ Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J (2007). "A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy". J Gen Intern Med. 22 (10): 1434–8. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0298-x. PMC 2305857. PMID 17653807.
{{cite journal}}
: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) - ^ Boyle M; et al. (2010). "Levels of empathy in undergraduate health science students". The Internet Journal of Medical Education. 1 (1).
- ^ Clay; Zanna (November 5, 2014). "Development of socio-emotional competence in bonobos". US: National Academy of Sciences. 110 (45): 18121–18126. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11018121C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1316449110.
- ^ Romero; Teresa (July 6, 2010). "Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern among chimpanzees". US: National Academy of Sciences. 107 (27): 12110–12115. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10712110R. doi:10.1073/pnas.1006991107. Retrieved February 15, 2014.
- ^ Hollis, Karen (March 2013). "A comparative analysis of precision rescue behaviour in sand-dwelling ants". British Journal of Animal Behaviour. Animal Behaviour. 85 (3): 537–544. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.005. Retrieved February 15, 2014.
- ^ Custance, Deborah; Jennifer Mayer (May 29, 2012). "Empathetic-like responding by domestic dogs (canis familiaris) to distress in humans". Animal Cognition. 15 (851–859): 851–859. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0510-1. PMID 22644113. Retrieved October 5, 2014.
- ^ Edgar, J; Paul (August 2013). "Protective Mother Hens: Cognitive influences on the avian maternal response". British Journal of Animal Behavior. 86 (2): 223–229. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.004. Retrieved March 6, 2014.
External links
- The dictionary definition of empathy at Wiktionary
- Quotations related to Empathy at Wikiquote
- Media related to Empathy at Wikimedia Commons
- "Empathy and Sympathy in Ethics". Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
- Entry on empathy at Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy