Jump to content

Close central rounded vowel

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

This is an old revision of this page, as edited by InternetArchiveBot (talk | contribs) at 17:02, 9 August 2017 (Rescuing 1 sources and tagging 1 as dead. #IABot (v1.5beta)). The present address (URL) is a permanent link to this revision, which may differ significantly from the current revision.

Close central rounded vowel
ʉ
ü
IPA number318
Encoding
Entity (decimal)ʉ
Unicode (hex)U+0289
X-SAMPA}
Braille⠴ (braille pattern dots-356)⠥ (braille pattern dots-136)

The close central rounded vowel, or high central rounded vowel,[1] is a type of vowel sound used in some spoken languages. The symbol in the International Phonetic Alphabet that represents this sound is ʉ, and the equivalent X-SAMPA symbol is }. Both the symbol and the sound are commonly referred to as "barred u".

The close central rounded vowel is the vocalic equivalent of the rare labialized post-palatal approximant [ẅ].[2]

In most languages this rounded vowel is pronounced with protruded lips (endolabial). However, in a few cases the lips are compressed (exolabial).

There is also a near-close central rounded vowel in some languages.

Close central protruded vowel

The close central protruded vowel is typically transcribed in IPA simply as ʉ, and that is the convention used in this article. As there is no dedicated diacritic for protrusion in the IPA, symbol for the close central rounded vowel with an old diacritic for labialization,   ̫, can be used as an ad hoc symbol ʉ̫ for the close central protruded vowel. Another possible transcription is ʉʷ or ɨʷ (a close central vowel modified by endolabialization), but this could be misread as a diphthong.

Features

  • Its vowel height is close, also known as high, which means the tongue is positioned close to the roof of the mouth without creating a constriction that would be classified as a consonant.
  • Its vowel backness is central, which means the tongue is positioned halfway between a front vowel and a back vowel.
  • Its roundedness is protruded, which means that the corners of the lips are drawn together, and the inner surfaces exposed.

Occurrence

Note: Because central rounded vowels are assumed to have protrusion, and few descriptions cover the distinction, some of the following may actually have compression.

Language Word IPA Meaning Notes
Angami Khonoma[3] [su] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [sʉ⁴] 'deep' Allophone of /u/ after /s/.[3]
Armenian Some Eastern dialects[4] [[[Armenian alphabet|յուղ]]] Error: {{Lang}}: unrecognized language code: arm (help) [jʉʁ] 'oil' Allophone of /u/ after /j/.
Berber Ayt Seghrouchen[5] ? [lːæjˈɡːʉɾ] 'he goes' Allophone of /u/ after velar consonants.
English Australian[6] [choose] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [t͡ʃʉːz] 'choose' In Australian English it's fronted [ʉ̟ː]. In Cockney and Estuary English it's often a diphthong [ʊʉ̯~əʉ̯]. In Scotland and the Scouse accent it can be more front, while in Geordie it can be more back. The exact length also varies between dialects. See Australian English phonology, English phonology, New Zealand English phonology and South African English phonology
Central Eastern American[7]
Cockney[8]
Estuary[9]
Modern RP speakers[10]
New Zealand[11]
Norfolk[12]
Scottish[13]
Scouse[14]
Some speakers of Geordie[15]
South African[16]
Southern American[17]
Ulster[18] Long allophone of /u/.[18] See English phonology
German Chemnitz dialect[19] [Buden] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [ˈpʉːtn̩] 'booths' See Chemnitz dialect phonology
Hausa[20] [example needed] Allophone of /u/.[20]
Ibibio Dialect of the Uruan area and Uyo[21] fuuk [fʉ́ʉk] 'cover many things/times' Allophone of /u/ between consonants.[21]
Some dialects[21] [example needed] Phonemic; contrasts with /u/.[21]
Irish Munster[22] ciúin [cʉ̠ːnʲ] 'quiet' Somewhat retracted; allophone of /u/ between slender consonants.[22] See Irish phonology
Ulster[23] úllaí [ʉ̜ɫ̪i][stress?] 'apples' Often only weakly rounded;[23] may be transcribed in IPA with u.
Lüsu[24] [lʉ5553] 'Lüsu'
Russian[25] кюрий [ˈkʲʉrʲɪj] 'curium' Allophone of /u/ between palatalized consonants. See Russian phonology
Scots[26] [buit] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [bʉt] 'boot' May be near-front [ʏ] instead.[26]
Swedish Bohuslän[27] [[[Swedish alphabet|yla]]] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [²ʉᶻːlä] 'howl' A fricated vowel that corresponds to [y̫ː] in Central Standard Swedish.[27] See Swedish phonology
Närke[27]
Tamil[28] வால் [väːlʉ] 'tail' Epenthetic vowel inserted in colloquial speech after word-final liquids; can be unrounded [ɨ] instead.[28] See Tamil phonology

Close central compressed vowel

Close central compressed vowel
ÿ
ɏ
ɨ͡β̞
ɨᵝ

As there is no official diacritic for compression in the IPA, the centering diacritic is used with the front rounded vowel [y], which is normally compressed. Another possibility is ɏ, a centralized [y] by analogy with the other close central vowels. Other possible transcriptions are ɨ͡β̞ (simultaneous [ɨ] and labial compression) and ɨᵝ ([ɨ] modified with labial compression[29]).

Features

  • Its vowel height is close, also known as high, which means the tongue is positioned close to the roof of the mouth without creating a constriction that would be classified as a consonant.
  • Its vowel backness is central, which means the tongue is positioned halfway between a front vowel and a back vowel.
  • Its roundedness is compressed, which means that the margins of the lips are tense and drawn together in such a way that the inner surfaces are not exposed.

Occurrence

This vowel is typically transcribed in IPA with ʉ. It occurs in some dialects of Swedish, but see also close front compressed vowel. The close back vowels of Norwegian and Swedish are also compressed. See close back compressed vowel. Medumba has a compressed central vowel [ɨᵝ] where the corners of the mouth are not drawn together.[30]

Language Word IPA Meaning Notes
Japanese Some younger speakers[31] 空気/kūki [kÿːki] 'air' Near-back [] for other speakers.[31] See Japanese phonology
Norwegian Urban East[32][33] [hus] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [hÿːs] 'house' Typically transcribed in IPA with ʉː. Also described as near-front [y̠ː].[34] See Norwegian phonology
Swedish Some dialects [ful] Error: {{Lang}}: text has italic markup (help) [fÿːl] 'ugly' More front [ ~ ʏː] in Central Standard Swedish; typically transcribed in IPA as ʉː. See Swedish phonology

See also

References

  1. ^ While the International Phonetic Association prefers the terms "close" and "open" for vowel height, many linguists use "high" and "low".
  2. ^ Instead of "post-palatal", it can be called "retracted palatal", "backed palatal", "palato-velar", "pre-velar", "advanced velar", "fronted velar" or "front-velar".
  3. ^ a b Blankenship et al. (1993), p. 129.
  4. ^ Dum-Tragut (2009), p. 14.
  5. ^ Abdel-Massih (1971), p. 20.
  6. ^ Harrington, Cox & Evans (1997).
  7. ^ Jilka, Matthias. "North American English Dialects" (PDF). Stuttgart: Institut für Linguistik/Anglistik, University of Stuttgart. p. 12. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 April 2014. {{cite web}}: Unknown parameter |deadurl= ignored (|url-status= suggested) (help)
  8. ^ Matthews (1938), p. 78.
  9. ^ Przedlacka (2001), p. 42.
  10. ^ "Received Pronunciation Phonology".
  11. ^ Mannell, Cox & Harrington (2009).
  12. ^ Lodge (2009), p. 168.
  13. ^ Scobbie, Gordeeva & Matthews (2006), p. 7.
  14. ^ Watson (2007), p. 357.
  15. ^ Watt & Allen (2003), p. 269.
  16. ^ Lass (2002), p. 116.
  17. ^ Labov, Ash & Boberg (2006), p. ?.
  18. ^ a b Jilka, Matthias. "Irish English and Ulster English" (PDF). Stuttgart: Institut für Linguistik/Anglistik, University of Stuttgart. p. 6. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 April 2014.
  19. ^ Khan & Weise (2013), p. 236.
  20. ^ a b Schuh & Yalwa (1999), p. 90.
  21. ^ a b c d Urua (2004), p. 106.
  22. ^ a b Ó Sé (2000), p. ?.
  23. ^ a b Ní Chasaide (1999), p. 114.
  24. ^ Chirkova & Chen (2013), p. 75.
  25. ^ Jones & Ward (1969), pp. 67–68.
  26. ^ a b Stuart-Smith (2004), p. 54.
  27. ^ a b c Riad (2014), p. 21.
  28. ^ a b Keane (2004), p. 114.
  29. ^ e.g. in Flemming (2002) Auditory representations in phonology, p. 83.
  30. ^ [1]
  31. ^ a b Okada (1999), p. 118.
  32. ^ Strandskogen (1979), pp. 15, 21.
  33. ^ Popperwell (2010), pp. 16, 29.
  34. ^ Vanvik (1979), pp. 13, 18.

Bibliography